All posts by student

Right on the heels of a PhIII failure, Merck neuroscience doubles down with a new drug targeting tau – Endpoints News

Darryle Schoepp

Merck isnt letting its first big Phase III failure in Alzheimers get in the way of its ambitions in the field. The pharma giant just grabbed worldwide rights to a preclinical tau-targeted antibody, lining up a parallel shot at a prime suspect in the development of the memory-wasting ailment.

Merck $MRK struck its deal with Japans Teijin Pharma, including an upfront and milestones which it didnt disclose (and rarely does). Now its neuroscience R&D group will take over development, reserving a royalty split if this one ever makes it to the market.

Merck has the most advanced BACE drug verubecestat, which moves upstream to halt development of amyloid beta in development. Back in February Merck shuttered its EPOCH trial for verubecestat in mild-to-moderate Alzheimers after the external data monitoring committee concluded that the drug was a bust, with virtually no chance of success. But the pharma giant is continuing its work on the drug with a separate Phase III in very early stage prodromal patients.

While toxic clusters of amyloid beta in the brain have long been considered the most likely cause of the disease, there has been growing attention for tau as well. That in turn has helped generate a growing consensus that any company that wants to make an impact on the disease, after 15 long years of clinical failure, will need to come up with cocktail therapies that cover a variety of targets. The tau-abeta combo is the first step in that direction, though theres no real certainty yet on exactly which mechanisms should be targeted.

Teijin Pharma scientists have made important progress to advance this investigational anti-tau antibody to this stage of development, said Darryle Schoepp, vice president, neuroscience discovery, Merck Research Laboratories. Merck remains committed to developing meaningful therapeutic options for the treatment of Alzheimers and other neurological diseases.

News reports for those who discover, develop, and market drugs. Join 16,000+ biopharma pros who read Endpoints News articles by email every day. Free subscription.

Read this article:
Right on the heels of a PhIII failure, Merck neuroscience doubles down with a new drug targeting tau - Endpoints News

Neuroscience and intelligence – Wikipedia

Neuroscience and intelligence refers to the various neurological factors that are partly responsible for the variation of intelligence within a species or between different species. A large amount of research in this area has been focused on the neural basis of human intelligence. Historic approaches to study the neuroscience of intelligence consisted of correlating external head parameters, for example head circumference, to intelligence.[1] Post-mortem measures of brain weight and brain volume have also been used.[1] More recent methodologies focus on examining correlates of intelligence within the living brain using techniques such as magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), functional MRI (fMRI), electroencephalography (EEG), positron emission tomography and other non-invasive measures of brain structure and activity.[1]

Researchers have been able to identify correlates of intelligence within the brain and its functioning. These include overall brain volume,[2] grey matter volume,[3] white matter volume,[4] white matter integrity,[5] cortical thickness[3] and neural efficiency.[6] Although the evidence base for our understanding of the neural basis of human intelligence has increased greatly over the past 30 years, even more research is needed to fully understand it.[1]

The neural basis of intelligence has also been examined in animals such as primates, cetaceans and rodents.[7]

One of the main methods used to establish a relationship between intelligence and the brain is to use measures of Brain volume.[1] The earliest attempts at estimating brain volume were done using measures of external head parameters, such as head circumference,[1] however, such approximations proved to be inaccurate when estimating brain size.[1] More recent methodologies that were employed to study this relationship are post-mortem measures of brain weight and volume. However, such measures also proved to be somewhat inconclusive, yielding diverging results depending on sex, which hemisphere was examined and on the type of intelligence measured.[8]

The most widespread methodology in contemporary neuroscience to measure brain volume and size is MRI. MRI is a non-invasive technique used to study the brain structure and function (using fMRI) of living subjects.[1] Overall, larger brain size and volume is associated with better cognitive functioning and higher intelligence. The correlations range from 0.0 to as high as 0.6, and are predominantly positive.[1] The specific regions that show the most robust correlation between volume and intelligence are the frontal, temporal and parietal lobes of the brain.[9][10][11] Therefore, it can be safely concluded that larger brains predict greater intelligence.[12][13]

However, researchers have cautioned against oversimplifying this view. A meta-analytic review by McDaniel found that the correlation between Intelligence and in vivo brain size was larger for females (0.40) than for males (0.25).[14] The same study also found that the correlation between brain size and Intelligence differed for age within sex, with children showing smaller correlations.[14] Furthermore, the hypothesis has been put forward that the relationship between larger brain volumes and higher intelligence is facilitated not by the global increase of brain volume, but instead by the enlargement of selective parts of the brain associated with specific tasks.[9] For example, monolingual adolescents learning new words is displayed growth in gray matter density in bilateral posterior supramarginal gyri directly related to the number of words learned.[15] Similarly, learning to juggle increased grey matter volume in the occipito-temporal cortex for subjects who could not juggle previously,[16] indicating that brain volume is dependent on a large variety of things and not a perfect measure for intelligence.

Another point of caution is that while larger brain volume is associated with higher intelligence, this relationship only explains a modest amount of variance within the distribution of intelligence itself. The correlations reported between brain volume, brain size and intelligence only explain 12% to 36% of the variance in the distribution of intelligence.[8][9] The amount of variance explained by brain volume also depends on the type of intelligence measured.[8] Up to 36% of variance in verbal intelligence can be explained by brain volume, while only approximately 10% of variance in visuospatial intelligence can be explained by brain volume.[8] These caveats imply that there are other major factors influencing how intelligent an individual is apart from brain size.[1] In a large meta-analysis consisting of 88 studies Pietschnig et al. (2015) estimated the correlation between brain volume and intelligence to be about correlation coefficient of 0.24 which equates to 6% variance.[17] Researcher Jakob Pietschnig showed that this strength of the positive association of brain volume and IQ has been overestimated in the literature, but still remains robust. He has stated that "It is tempting to interpret this association in the context of human cognitive evolution and species differences in brain size and cognitive ability, we show that it is not warranted to interpret brain size as an isomorphic proxy of human intelligence differences".[17] Another 2015 study by researcher Stuart J Ritchie found that brain size explained 12% of the variance in intelligence among individuals.[18]

Grey matter has been examined as a potential biological foundation for differences in intelligence. Similarly to brain volume, global grey matter volume is positively associated with intelligence.[1] More specifically, higher intelligence has been associated with larger cortical grey matter in the prefrontal and posterior temporal cortex in adults.[3] Furthermore, both verbal and nonverbal intelligence have been shown to be positively correlated with grey matter volume across the parietal, temporal and occipital lobes in young healthy adults, implying that intelligence is associated with a wide variety of structures within the brain.[19]

There appear to be sex differences between the relationship of grey matter to intelligence between men and women.[20] Men appear to show more intelligence to grey matter correlations in the frontal and parietal lobes, while the strongest correlations between intelligence and grey matter in women can be found in the frontal lobes and Broca's area.[20] However, these differences do not seem to impact overall Intelligence, implying that the same cognitive ability levels can be attained in different ways.[20]

One specific methodology used to study grey matter correlates of intelligence in areas of the brain is known as voxel-based morphometry (VBM). VBM allows researchers to specify areas of interest with great spatial resolution, allowing the examination of grey matter areas correlated with intelligence with greater special resolution. VBM has been used to correlate grey matter positively with intelligence in the frontal, temporal, parietal, and occipital lobes in healthy adults.[21] VBM has also been used to show that grey matter volume in the medial region of the prefrontal cortex and the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex correlate positively with intelligence in a group of 55 healthy adults.[22] VBM has also been successfully used to establish a positive correlation between grey matter volumes in the anterior cingulate and intelligence in children aged 5 to 18 years old.[23]

Grey matter has also been shown to positively correlate with intelligence in children.[23][24][25] Reis and colleagues[25] have found that grey matter in the prefrontal cortex contributes most robustly to variance in Intelligence in children between 5 and 17, while subcortical grey matter is related to intelligence to a lesser extent. Frangou and colleagues[24] examined the relationship between grey matter and intelligence in children and young adults aged between 12 and 21, and found that grey matter in the orbitofrontal cortex, cingulate gyrus, cerebellum and thalamus was positively correlated to intelligence, while grey matter in the caudate nucleus is negatively correlated with intelligence. However, the relationship between grey matter volume and intelligence only develops over time, as no significant positive relationship can be found between grey matter volume and intelligence in children under 11.[23]

An underlying caveat to research into the relationship of grey matter volume and intelligence is demonstrated by the hypothesis of neural efficiency.[6][26] The findings that more intelligent individuals are more efficient at using their neurons might indicate that the correlation of grey matter to intelligence reflects selective elimination of unused synapses, and thus a better brain circuitry.[27]

Similar to grey matter, white matter has been shown to correlate positively with intelligence in humans.[1][4] White matter consists mainly of myelinated neuronal axons, responsible for delivering signals between neurons. The pinkish-white color of white matter is actually a result of these myelin sheaths that electrically insulate neurons that are transmitting signals to other neurons. White matter connects different regions of grey matter in the cerebrum together.These interconnections make transport more seamless and allow us to perform tasks easier. Significant correlations between intelligence and the corpus callosum have been found, as larger callosal areas have been positively correlated with cognitive performance.[1] However, there appear to be differences in importance for white matter between verbal and nonverbal intelligence, as although both verbal and nonverbal measures of intelligence correlate positively with the size of the corpus callosum, the correlation for intelligence and corpus callosum size was larger (.47) for nonverbal measures than that for verbal measures (.18).[28] Anatomical mesh-based geometrical modelling[29][30][31] has also shown positive correlations between the thickness of the corpus callosum and Intelligence in healthy adults.[32]

White matter integrity has also been found to be related to Intelligence.[5] White matter tract integrity is important for information processing speed, and therefore reduced white matter integrity is related to lower intelligence.[5] The effect of white matter integrity is mediate entirely through information processing speed.[5] These findings indicate that the brain is structurally interconnected and that axonal fibres are integrally important for fast information process, and thus general intelligence.[5]

Contradicting the findings described above, VBM failed to find a relationship between the corpus callosum and intelligence in healthy adults.[21] This contradiction can be viewed to signify that the relationship between white matter volume and intelligence is not as robust as that of grey matter and intelligence.[1]

Cortical thickness has also been found to correlate positively with intelligence in humans.[3] However, the rate of growth of cortical thickness is also related to intelligence.[27] In early childhood, cortical thickness displays a negative correlation with intelligence, while by late childhood this correlation has shifted to a positive one.[27] More intelligent children were found to develop cortical thickness more steadily and over longer periods of time than less bright children.[27] Studies have found cortical thickness to explain 5% in the variance of intelligence among individuals.[18] In a study conducted to find associations between cortical thickness and general intelligence between different groups of people, sex did not play a role in intelligence.[33] Although it is hard to pin intelligence on age based on cortical thickness due to different socioeconomic circumstances and education levels, older subjects (17 - 24) tended to have less variances in terms of intelligence than when compared to younger subjects (19 - 17).[33]

Cortical convolution has increased the folding of the brains surface over the course of human evolution. It has been hypothesized that the high degree of cortical convolution may be a neurological substrate that supports some of the human brain's most distinctive cognitive abilities. Consequently, individual intelligence within the human species might be modulated by the degree of cortical convolution.[34]

The neural efficiency hypothesis postulates that more intelligent individuals display less activation in the brain during cognitive tasks, as measured by Glucose metabolism.[6] A small sample of participants (N=8) displayed negative correlations between intelligence and absolute regional metabolic rates ranging from -0.48 to -0.84, as measured by PET scans, indicating that brighter individuals were more effective processors of information, as they use less energy.[6] According to an extensive review by Neubauer & Fink[35] a large number of studies (N=27) have confirmed this finding using methods such as PET scans,[36] EEG[37] and fMRI.[38] However, evidence contradicting the neural efficiency hypothesis also exists.[35]

fMRI and EEG studies have revealed that task difficulty is an important factor affecting neural efficiency.[35] More intelligent individuals display neural efficiency only when faced with tasks of subjectively easy to moderate difficulty, while no neural efficiency can be found during difficult tasks.[39] In fact, more able individuals appear to invest more cortical resources in tasks of high difficulty.[35] This appears to be especially true for the Prefrontal Cortex, as individuals with higher intelligence displayed increased activation of this area during difficult tasks compared to individuals with lower intelligence.[40][41] It has been proposed that the main reason for the neural efficiency phenomenon could be that individuals with high intelligence are better at blocking out interfering information than individuals with low intelligence.[42]

Some scientists prefer to look at more qualitative variables to relate to the size of measurable regions of known function, for example relating the size of the primary visual cortex to its corresponding functions, that of visual performance.[43][44]

In a study of the head growth of 633 term-born children from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children cohort, it was shown that prenatal growth and growth during infancy were associated with subsequent IQ. The studys conclusion was that the brain volume a child achieves by the age of 1 year helps determine later intelligence. Growth in brain volume after infancy may not compensate for poorer earlier growth.[45]

There is an association between IQ and myopia. One suggested explanation is that one or several pleiotropic gene(s) affect the size of the neocortex part of the brain and eyes simultaneously.[46]

In 2007, Behavioral and Brain Sciences published a target article that put forth a biological model of intelligence based on 37 peer-reviewed neuroimaging studies (Jung & Haier, 2007). Their review of a wealth of data from functional imaging (functional magnetic resonance imaging and positron emission tomography) and structural imaging (diffusion MRI, voxel-based morphometry, in vivo magnetic resonance spectroscopy) argues that human intelligence arises from a distributed and integrated neural network comprising brain regions in the frontal and parietal lobes.[47]

A recent lesion mapping study conducted by Barbey and colleagues provides evidence to support the P-FIT theory of intelligence.[48][49][50]

Brain injuries at an early age isolated to one side of the brain typically results in relatively spared intellectual function and with IQ in the normal range.[51]

Another theory of brain size in vertebrates is that it may relate to social rather than mechanical skill. Cortical size relates directly to a pairbonding life style and among primates cerebral cortex size varies directly with the demands of living in a large complex social network. Compared to other mammals, primates have significantly larger brain size. Additionally, most primates are found to be polygynandrous, having many social relationships with others. Although inconclusive, some studies have shown that this polygnandrous statue correlates to brain size.[52]

Several environmental factors related to health can lead to significant cognitive impairment, particularly if they occur during pregnancy and childhood when the brain is growing and the bloodbrain barrier is less effective. Developed nations have implemented several health policies regarding nutrients and toxins known to influence cognitive function. These include laws requiring fortification of certain food products and laws establishing safe levels of pollutants (e.g. lead, mercury, and organochlorides). Comprehensive policy recommendations targeting reduction of cognitive impairment in children have been proposed.[53]

See the article here:
Neuroscience and intelligence - Wikipedia

98-year-old neuroscience pioneer gives no thought to slowing down – The Columbus Dispatch

By Benedict CareyThe New York Times

MONTREAL At 98,Brenda Milner is not letting up in a nearly 70-year career to clarify the function of many brain regions frontal lobes, and temporal; vision centers and tactile; the left hemisphere and the right usually by painstakingly testing people with brain lesions, often from surgery.

Her prominence long ago transcended gender, and she is impatient with those who expect her to be a social activist. Its science first with Milner, say close colleagues,in her lab and her life.

Milner, a professor of psychology in the department of neurology and neurosurgery at McGill University in Montreal, is best known for discovering the seat of memory in the brain, the foundational finding of cognitive neuroscience. But she also has a knack for picking up on subtle quirks of human behavior and linking them to brain function.

Perched recently on a chair in her small office, resplendent in a black satin dress and gold floral pin and banked by moldering towers of old files, she volleyed questions rather than answering them.

People think because Im 98 years old I must be emerita, she said. Well, not at all. Im still nosy, you know, curious.

Milner continues working because she sees no reason not to. Neither McGill nor the affiliated Montreal Neurological Institute and Hospital has asked her to step aside.

She has funding: In 2014, she won three prominent achievement awards that came with money for research. She has a project: a continuing study to investigate how the healthy brains intellectual left hemisphere coordinates with its more aesthetic right one in thinking and memory.

And she has adapted to the life as an undeniably "senior" senior researcher. I come into the office about three days a week or so, that is plenty, Milner said.

And I have some rules, she added. I will take on postdoctoral students, but not graduate students. Graduate students need to know youll be around for five years or so, and well she chuckled, looking up at the ceiling well, its very difficult if they have to switch to someone else, you know.

Milners current project is, appropriately enough, an attempt to weave together two of brain sciences richest strands of research, both of which she helped originate a lifetime ago.

One is the biology of memory.

Milner changed the course of brain science for good as a newly minted Ph.D. in the 1950s by identifying the specific brain organ that is crucial to memory formation.

She did so by observing the behavior of a 29-year-old Connecticut man who had recently undergone an operation to relieve severe epileptic seizures. The operation was an experiment: On a hunch, the surgeon suctioned out two trenches of tissue from the mans brain, one from each of his medial temporal lobes, located deep below the skull about level with the ears. The seizures subsided.

But the patient, an assembly line worker named Henry Molaison, was forever altered. He could no longer form new memories.

Concerned and intrigued, the surgeon contacted researchers Wilder Penfield and Milner at the Montreal Neurological Institute, who had previously reported on two cases of amnesia in patients treated there. Thus began a now-famous collaboration.

She started taking the night train from Montreal to give a battery of tests to Molaison, who was known in research reports as H.M. to protect his privacy.

In a landmark 1957 paper, Milner wrote with Molaisons surgeon, she concluded that the medial temporal areas including, importantly, an organ called the hippocampus must be critical to memory formation. That finding, though slow to sink in, upended the accepted teaching at the time, which held that no single area was critical to supporting memory.

Milner continued to work with Molaison and later showed that his motor memory was intact: He remembered how to perform certain physical drawing tests even if he had no memory of having learned them.

The finding, reported in 1962, demonstrated that there are at least two systems in the brain for processing memory: one that is explicit and handles names, faces and experiences; and another that is implicit and incorporates skills, like riding a bike or playing a guitar.

I clearly remember to this day my excitement, sitting there with H.M. and watching this beautiful learning curve develop right there in front of me, Milner said. I knew very well I was witnessing something important.

The other strand her new research project incorporates is hemispheric specialization: how the brains two halves, the right and the left, divide its mental labor.

The new project is aimed at understanding how hemispheric coordination aids memory retrieval under normal circumstances, in people without brain injuries. Milner leads a research team that has been taking exhaustive MRI brain images from participants while they solve problems and take memory tests.

Does the artistic right hemisphere provide clues to help its more logic-oriented other half retrieve words? If so, which kinds of clues seem most powerful?

In one experiment, participants in the brain scanner tried to recall a list of words they had just studied. Some of those words were concrete, like dog or house, conjuring specific imagery; others, like concept or strategy, were not. The scans carefully track activation across hemispheres moment to moment, as retrieval happens. The findings hold tremendous potential to help people with early dementia, some brain injuries and even learning disabilities.

People with early signs of dementia can have trouble with imagery, and by the time the disease is advanced, theyve lost that ability, said Joelle Crane, a clinical psychologist at the Montreal Neurological Institute. One area this new work might help us with is in training people to learn in a more visual way.

For Milner, after a lifetime exploring the brain, the motive for the work is personal as well as professional. I live very close; its a 10-minute walk up the hill, she said. So it gives me a good reason to come in regularly.

Read more:
98-year-old neuroscience pioneer gives no thought to slowing down - The Columbus Dispatch

Lab Tube Meets Prof. Andrea Brand at the BNA Festival of Neuroscience 2017 – Technology Networks

We caught up with Prof. Andrea Brand at the BNA Neuro Fest in Birmingham, UK, and asked her about her research investigating nutritional modulation of neural stem cells.

Andrea also explains how her lab employ their DamID technique to investigate protein-DNA interaction and chromatin changes in vivo.

To finish, Andrea highlights some of the reserach challenges her lab hope to overcome in the future.

Like what you just watched? You can find similar content on the communities below.

To personalize the content you see on Technology Networks homepage, Log In or Subscribe for Free

Excerpt from:
Lab Tube Meets Prof. Andrea Brand at the BNA Festival of Neuroscience 2017 - Technology Networks

Facebook establishes neuroscience centre dedicated to marketing studies – The Drum

Facebook is branching out into the field of neuroscience research with the creation of its own scientific lab dedicated to devising new marketing techniques for agencies, brands and media firms.

The Center for Marketing Science Innovation is still under construction in Manhattan but Facebooks director of advertising research gave Ad Week a sneak peak of the fledgling facility, which takes the underwhelming appearance of a GPs surgery.

This unassuming facade bellies a hi-tech operation behind the scenes however with neuroscience specialists to help guide marketers, publishers and brands toward impactful content.

To achieve this Facebook has built a variety of rooms designed to mimic common viewing environments such as a living area or conference suite with an array of monitors tracking every twitch and glance of strapped in guinea pigs.

By keeping tabs on heart rate, facial movements and eye movements of participants as they scroll through profiles or consume TV content it is hoped to gain a better understanding of how such imperceptible biological reactions correspond to real world behavior.

Speaking to Ad Week Daniel Slotwiner, Facebooks director of advertising research, explained: A lot of what we dont understand is where peoples eyes are going when theyre on the platform. We know how much time people are spending on the platform, so this is really about how that time is spent and what features on our product theyre looking at.

Facebook isnt the first organisation to take an interest in biological cues for reading engagement, with a landmark Imperial College London study enabling marketers to get to know us better than we know ourselves.

The new centre is expected to be finished within the next few weeks.

More here:
Facebook establishes neuroscience centre dedicated to marketing studies - The Drum

A Stanford scientist on the biology of human evil – Vox

What drives human behavior? Why do we do what we do? Is free will an illusion? Has civilization made us better? Can we escape our tribal past?

These questions (and many, many others) are the subject of a new book called Behave: The Biology of Humans at Our Best and Worst. The author is Robert Sapolsky, a biology professor at Stanford and a research associate with the Institute of Primate Research at the National Museums of Kenya.

In a brisk 800 pages, Sapolsky covers nearly every facet of the human condition, engaging moral philosophy, evolutionary biology, social science, and genetics along the way.

The key question of the book why are we the way we are? is explored from a multitude of angles, and the narrative structure helps guide the reader. For instance, Sapolsky begins by examining a persons behavior in the moment (why we recoil or rejoice or respond aggressively to immediate stimuli) and then zooms backward in time, following the chain of antecedent causes back to our evolutionary roots.

For every action, Sapolsky shows, there are several layers of causal significance: Theres a neurobiological cause and a hormonal cause and a chemical cause and a genetic cause, and, of course, there are always environmental and historical factors. He synthesizes the research across these disciplines into a coherent, readable whole.

In this interview, I talk with Sapolsky about the paradoxes of human nature, why were capable of both good and evil, whether free will exists, and why symbols have become so central to human life.

This conversation has been edited for length and clarity.

You start the book with a paradox of sorts: Humans are both exceptionally violent and exceptionally kind. Were capable on the one hand of mass genocide, and on the other hand of heroic self-sacrifice. How do we make sense of this dichotomy?

In an evolutionary sense, we're this incredibly confused species, in between all sorts of extremes of behavior and patterns of selection compared to other primates who are far more consistently X or Y, and we're so often floating in between. In a more proximal sense, I think what that tells you over and over again is just how important context is.

Can you clarify what you mean by context here?

Sure. What counts as our worst and best behaviors are so much in the eye of the beholder. So often it really is the one man's freedom fighter versus the other's terrorist. But even separate of that, just the fact that in some settings our biology is such that we are extraordinarily prosocial creatures, and in other settings extraordinarily antisocial creatures, shows how important it is to really understand the biology of our response to context and environment.

You argue that biological factors don't so much cause behavior as modulate it can you explain what you mean?

Ultimately, there is no debate. Insofar as using "genes" as a surrogate for "nature," it only makes sense to ask what a gene does in a particular environment, and to ask what the behavioral effects of an environment are given someone's genetic makeup. They're inseparable in a way that is most meaningful when it comes to humans.

Given how variable human behavior is, do you believe in a fixed human nature? There is a lot of debate about this in the world of philosophy. I wonder how you think about it as a scientist.

Human nature is extraordinarily malleable, and I think that's the most defining thing about our nature.

Okay, but in the book you come awfully close to concluding something very different. Specifically, in your discussion of free will, you reluctantly embrace a deterministic account of human behavior. You argue that free will is, in fact, an illusion, and if thats true, Im not sure how malleable we can be.

If it seemed tentative, it was just because I was trying to be polite to the reader or to a certain subset of readers. If there is free will, its free will about all sorts of uninteresting stuff, and it's getting cramped into tighter and increasingly boring places. It seems impossible to view the full range of influences on our behavior and conclude that there is anything like free will.

Thats a bold claim...

Youre right. On the one hand, it seems obvious to me and to most scientists thinking about behavior that there is no free will. And yet its staggeringly difficult to try to begin to even imagine what a world is supposed to look like in which everybody recognizes this and accepts this.

The most obvious place to start is to approach this differently in terms of how we judge behavior. Even an extremely trivial decision like the shirt you choose to wear today, if dissected close enough, doesnt really involve agency in the way we assume. There are millions of antecedent causes that led you to choose that shirt, and you had no control over them. So if I was to compliment you and say, Hey, nice shirt, that doesnt really make any sense in that you arent really responsible for wearing it, at least not in the way that question implies.

Now, this is a very trivial thing and doesnt appear to matter much, but this logic is also true for serious and consequential behaviors, and thats where things get complicated.

If we're just marionettes on a string and we don't have the kind of agency that we think we have, then what sense does it make to reward or punish behavior? Doesnt that imply some degree of freedom of action?

Organisms on the average tend to increase the frequency of behaviors for which theyve been rewarded and to do the opposite for punishment or absence of reward. That's fine and instrumentally is going to be helpful in all sorts of circumstances. The notion of there being something virtuous about punishing a bad behavior, that's the idea thats got to go out the window.

I always come back to the example of epilepsy. Five hundred years ago, an epileptic seizure was a sign that you were hanging out with Satan, and the appropriate treatment for that was obvious: burning someone at the stake. This went on for hundreds of years. Now, of course, we know that such a person has got screwy potassium channels in their neurons. It's not them; it's a disease. It's not a moral failing; it's a biological phenomenon.

Now we dont punish epileptics for their epilepsy, but if they suffer bouts frequently, we might not let them drive a car because its not safe. Its not that they dont deserve to drive a car; its that its not safe. Its a biological thing that has to be constrained because it represents a danger.

Its taken us 500 years or so to get to this revelation, so I dont know how long it will take us to reach this mindset for all other sorts of behaviors, but we absolutely must get there.

So what is true for the epileptic is true for all of us all of the time? We are our brains and we had no role in the shaping of our biology or our neurology or our chemistry, and yet these are the forces that determine our behavior.

Thats true, but its still difficult to fully grasp this. Look, I believe there is no free will whatsoever, but I can't function that way. I get pissed off at our dog if he pees on the floor in the kitchen, even though I can easily come up with a mechanistic explanation for that.

Our entire notion of moral and legal responsibility is thrown into doubt the minute we fully embrace this truth, so Im not sure we can really afford to own up to the implications of free will being an illusion.

I think thats mostly right. As individuals and a society, Im not sure were ready to face this fact. But we could perhaps do it bits and pieces at a time.

You write that our species has problems with violence. Can you explain this complicated relationship?

The easiest answer is that we're really violent. The much more important one, the much more challenging one, is that we don't hate violence as such we hate the wrong kind of violence, and when it's the right kind of violence, we absolutely do cartwheels to reinforce it and reward it and hand out medals and mate with such people because of it. And thats part of the reason why the worst kinds of violence are so viscerally awful to experience, to bear witness to. But the right kinds of violence are just as visceral, only in the opposite direction.

The truth is that this is the hardest realm of human behavior to understand, but its also the most important one to try to.

What is the wrong kind of violence? What is the right kind of violence?

Of course that tends to be in the eye of the beholder. Far too often, the right kind is one that fosters the fortunes of people just like us in group favoritism, and the worst kinds are the ones that do the opposite.

Violence is a fact of nature all species engage in it one way or other. Are humans the only species that ritualizes it, that makes a sport of it?

That does seem pretty much the case. Certainly you see the hints of it in chimps, for example, where you see order patrols by male chimps in one group, where if they encounter a male from another group, they will kill him. They have now been shown in a number of circumstances to have systematically killed all the males in the neighboring group, which certainly fits a rough definition of genocide, which is to say killing an individual not because of what they did but simply because of what group they belong to.

What's striking with the chimps is that you can tell beforehand that this is where they are heading. They do something vaguely ritualistic, which is they do a whole bunch of emotional contagion stuff. One male gets very agitated, very aroused, manages to get others like that, and then off they go to look for somebody to attack. So in that regard, there is a ritualistic feel to it, but that's easily framed along the conventional lines of nonhuman animal violence. By that, I mean when male chimps do this, when they eradicate all of the other males in a neighboring territory, they expand their own; it increases their reproductive success.

I believe it is really only humans that do violence for purely ritualistic purposes.

Is our tribal past the most important thing to understand about human behavior?

I think it's an incredibly important one, and what's most important about it is to understand the implications of the fact that all of us have multiple tribal affiliations that we carry in our heads and to understand the circumstances that bring one of those affiliations to the forefront over another. The mere fact that you can switch people's categorization of others from race to religion to what sports team they follow speaks to how incredibly complicated and central tribal affiliation is to humans and to human life.

You spend a lot of time talking about the role of symbols and ideas in human life. We kill and we die for our symbols, and we often confuse the symbols themselves for the things they symbolize. Do you think symbols and ideas amplify our tribal nature, or do they help us transcend it?

Well, its important to understand that not only are we willing to kill people because they look, dress, eat things, smell, speak, sing, pray differently from us, but also because they have incredibly different ideas as to very abstract notions. I think the thing that fuels that capacity is how primitively our brains do symbolism.

I think the fact that our brains so readily intermix the abstractions and symbols with their visceral, metaphorical analogues gives those abstractions and symbols enormous power. That fact that were willing to kill and die for abstract symbols is itself crazy, but nonetheless true.

Has civilization made us better?

Absolutely. The big question is which of the following two scenarios are more correct: a) Civilization has made us the most peaceful, cooperative, emphatic we've ever been as a species, versus b) civilization is finally inching us back to the level of all those good things that characterized most of hominin hunter-gatherer history, preceding the invention of agriculture. Amid mostly being an academic outsider to the huge debates over this one, I find the latter view much more convincing.

You say you incline to pessimism but that this book gave you reasons to be optimistic. Why?

Because there's very little about our behaviors that are inevitable, including our worst behaviors. And were learning more and more about the biological underpinnings of our behavior, and that can help us produce better outcomes. As long as you have a ridiculously long view of things, things are getting better.

Its much nicer to be alive today than it was 100 or 200 years ago, and thats because weve progressed. But nothing is certain, and we have to continue moving forward if we want to preserve what progress weve made.

Read the rest here:
A Stanford scientist on the biology of human evil - Vox

Altoona student’s interest in behavior leads to research on humans and zebrafish – Penn State News

ALTOONA, Pa. Why do people behave in the way they do? Put three people in the same situation and there is no guarantee that all three will react in the same manner. Coming upon a car accident, one might stop, maybe grabbing a blanket out of the car to help the injured. Another might just call 911 and wait for professional help. The third person might just drive on, not wishing to get involved. But why?

The study of psychology helps answer that and many other questions about human behavior. And thats what drew Penn State Altoona student John Leri to change his major.

Leri enrolled at Penn State Altoona intending to be a business major. But then he started working with Samantha Tornello, assistant professor of psychology and womens, gender, and sexuality studies, and, he said, I realized that the research that surrounded psychology was what I was interested in the culture of science, the process of science. You get to ask a question and then try to answer it. So he switched majors.

I felt I would have a more well-rounded view of the world if I focused on how people behave as opposed to how they act in a business setting. It felt right once I was there," he said.

Due to his hard work, Leri was enrolled in the Schreyer Honors College, where one of the requirements is writing a thesis. Because I was interested in research I did an independent study on depression, specifically looking at how people interact with depressed people, he said.

Exploring the concept of social distance, defined as an individuals willingness to associate (or not) with another person, Leri recruited 425 participants and found that people were equally willing to interact with those with depression whether or not the people with depression were taking antidepressants.

Those who held greater stigmatizing beliefs regarding depression, greater social-dominance orientation, and less personal exposure to mental illness reported wanting greater social distance to the individual diagnosed with depression, regardless of treatment status," said Leri. "These results suggest that both mental illness exposure and depression-related stigma can be useful areas of interventions to reduce negative attitudes toward individuals diagnosed with depression.

At the same time Leri was researching and writing his thesis, he was employed as a research assistant in Tornellos lab, so she heard regular updates on his progress. She was impressed with his dedication and passion. He talked about it in great depth, Tornello said. He took the reins spearheading the data collection and data analysis, and writing the manuscript. His efforts, she acknowledged, were equal to students in graduate school.

Lynn Nagle, instructor in psychology and education, first had Leri in some psychology classes and then served as his faculty adviser for his Psych 495 internship. Somewhere along the way, she said, I recruited his involvement with the Psychology Club. Being John, he quickly realized if he was going to be involved with Psychology Club, he was going to run the show, and that he did.

Leri was elected president of the club for the 201516 academic year and Nagle said he was a great asset: He generated novel ideas and increased attendance and participation at Psychology Club events.He was truly invaluable to me as an officer.

The internship Leri had was at NPC, Inc., a document processing service in Roaring Springs, where he helped to refine their job application process. Nagle said, When I supervised his internship based in the field of industrial organization psychology, he was not only focused on improving the employee work environment and making the training more effective, he was truly interested in what drove the employees; he seemed to be trying to decipher employee internal motivation.

In his senior year Leri worked on research projects for two more Penn State Altoona faculty: Cairsty DePasquale and Lara LaDage, both assistant professors of biology. DePasquales research fits well with Leris interest in why people behave the way they do.

Anecdotal evidence shows exercise can reduce anxiety and depression, DePasquale said, noting human studies where exercise regimes can reduce anxiety. Similar results occur with animal models.

To study the effects of exercise on zebrafish, DePasquale had Leri use a swim tunnel built by engineers on campus.

This brings questions, such as how does one exercise a fish? (The swim tunnel) uses channels. We vary the flow of water in the channels and get fish to swim against the flow of water, DePasquale explained.

So how does one test for anxiety in fish? DePasquale said, Its very similar to tests on rodents but we have to adapt to an aquatic environment. We put them in a novel tank with nothing else and the fish will stay at the bottom of the tank. As they begin to explore the tank more, they are more willing to move out of their comfort zone. Fish who move up are less anxious fish. Another test was the light/dark test; fish tend to shy away from bright lights, as do rodents. We use that avoidance to look at anxiety behavior.

How did Leri like working with the zebrafish? Working with animals is a pain in the butt, he said, injecting a little humor into what is serious research. With humans you can at least give them instructions. Getting (the fish) to read the instructions is the hard part.

LaDage said, John joined my and Cairstys lab at the same time, which speaks to his interest in, and dedication to, pursuing research opportunities. In my lab he was instrumental in collecting data and writing up a manuscript concerning the assessment of substructural changes in the brain.

Even though Leri graduated in August 2016 he continued the research work while considering continuing his education in graduate school. In January 2017 Leri presented his research at the Society for Integrative and Comparative Biology meeting in New Orleans.

Leri described their research as follows: The lizard hippocampal equivalent, the medial cortex, is used to study environmental impact on neural tissue and spatial memory. Although the cortex is comprised of three substrates, each with differentiating traits in cellular architecture, studies typically use overall volume as an outcome variable. Our research showed that overall volume may not accurately represent changes taking place within cortical substrates.

Leri attended the Eastern Psychological Association conference in Boston in March and presented a poster based on his honors thesis and his work with Tornello. It discusses the predictive role of negative attitudes associated with depression and willingness to interact with an individual diagnosed with depression, he explained.

Nagle echoed Leri's other professors when she said, John is one of those stellar students, who you wish you could replicate. Hes cordial, respectful and has an incredible work ethic. He has a great sense of humor and is very interesting to talk with. He has such a wide range of experiences, I think anyone could find something in common with him and once you get him talking, he just lights up. I cant wait to see where John goes to grad school and how accomplished he will become.

Leri has now chosen his path. Beginning in the fall of 2017 he will attend the University of Florida to work under Darlene Kertes. Once again, hell be tackling more than one subject. I have been admitted into the behavioral and cognitive neuroscience (psychology) program," he said.

Based on his history of taking on more than one project at a time at Penn State Altoona, its no surprise that hes planning to pursue a dual doctorate in behavior and cognitive neuroscience and social psychology. A long way from that freshman business major but, for John Leri, definitely the right path.

Read more:
Altoona student's interest in behavior leads to research on humans and zebrafish - Penn State News

Improve evolution education by teaching genetics first – Phys.org – Phys.Org

May 23, 2017 Children taught genetics first increase their understanding of evolution. Credit: Miki Yoshihito, Flickr

Evolution is a difficult concept for many students at all levels, however, a study publishing on May 23 in the open access journal PLOS Biology has demonstrated a simple cost-free way to significantly improve students' understanding of evolution at the secondary level: teach genetics before you teach them evolution.

Currently in the UK setting the two modules are taught in isolation often with long time intervals between. The team, led by Professor Laurence Hurst at the Milner Centre for Evolution, University of Bath hypothesised that since core concepts of genetics (such as DNA and mutation) are so intimately linked to the core concepts of evolution, then priming students with genetics information might help their understanding of evolution.

The researchers conducted a large controlled trial of almost 2000 students aged 14-16 in 78 classes from 23 schools across the south and south west of the UK, in which teachers were asked to teach genetics before evolution or evolution before genetics.

The students were tested prior to teaching and after. The five year study, found that those taught genetics first improved their test scores by an average of seven per cent more than those taught evolution first.

Teaching genetics before evolution was particularly crucial for students in foundation classes, who increased their understanding of evolution only if they were taught genetics first. The higher ability classes saw an increase in evolution understanding with both orders, but it was greatest if genetics was taught first.

The team also tested the students' understanding of genetics and found that the genetics-first effect either increased genetics understanding as well or made no difference, meaning that teaching genetics first doesn't harm students' appreciation of this subject.

Professor Hurst, commented: "These are very exciting results. School teachers are under enormous pressure to do the best for their students but have little time to make changes and understandably dislike constant disruption to the curriculum."

"To be sensitive to their needs, in the trial we let teachers teach what they normally teach - we just looked at the order effect."

First author on the paper Dr Rebecca Mead, a former teacher herself, said: "It's remarkable that such a simple and cost-free intervention makes such a big difference. That genetics-first was the only intervention that worked for the foundation classes is especially important as these classes are often challenging to teach. This research has encouraged teachers to rethink how they teach evolution and genetics and many schools have now changed their teaching practice to genetics-first. I hope more will follow."

The team also looked at whether students in the study agreed or disagreed with the scientific view of evolution. They found that whilst the teaching of evolution increased acceptance rates to over 80 per cent in the cohort examined, the order of teaching had no effect.

Qualitative focus group follow-up studies showed that acceptance is heavily conditioned by authority figures (teachers, TV personalities, religious figures) and the correlation between the students' understanding of evolution and their acceptance of it is weak.

Dr. Mead commented: "Some students reported that being told that key authority figures approve of the scientific evidence for evolution made a big difference to their learning experience. It would be worth testing alternative ways to help students overcome preconceptions."

Explore further: Evolution and religion: New insight into instructor attitudes in Arizona

More information: Mead R, Hejmadi M, Hurst LD (2017) Teaching genetics prior to teaching evolution improves evolution understanding but not acceptance. PLoS Biol 15(5): e2002255. doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.2002255

Evolution can be an emotionally charged topic in education, given a wide range of perspectives on it. Two researchers from Arizona State University are taking an in-depth look at how college professors handle it.

College students' views about evolution and creationism are often shaped by what they learned in their high school biology classes, according to a University of Minnesota study published in the May issue of BioScience, the ...

University of Alabama at Birmingham (UAB) Associate Professor Lee Meadows, Ph.D., is author of a new book that claims it's possible to teach evolution without offending students who have strong religious convictions against ...

The Texas Board of Education will decide whether to scrap a requirement that public schools teach high school students to scrutinize "all sides" of scientific theory after hearing Tuesday from academics who say that was meant ...

High school and college students who understand the geological age of the Earth (4.5 billion years) are much more likely to understand and accept human evolution, according to a University of Minnesota study published in ...

South Dakota legislators are weighing whether to let teachers decide how much skepticism to work into lessons on contentious scientific topics such as evolution and climate change.

In human history, the transition from hunting and gathering to farming is a significant one. As such, hunter-gatherers and farmers are usually thought about as two entirely different sets of people. But researchers reporting ...

A new species of a fossil pliosaur (large predatory marine reptile from the 'age of dinosaur') has been found in Russia and profoundly change how we understand the evolution of the group, says an international team of scientists.

People using smartphones are more likely to make rational and unemotional decisions compared to PC users when presented with a moral dilemma on their device, according to a new study from City, University of London.

A-tisket, A-tasket. You can tell a lot from a basket. Especially if it comes from the ruins of an ancient civilization inhabited by humans nearly 15,000 years ago during the Late Pleistocene and Early Holocene ages.

Middle Stone Age humans in the Porc-Epic cave likely used ochre over at least 4,500 years, according to a study published May 24, 2017 in the open-access journal PLOS ONE by Daniela Rosso from the University of Barcelona, ...

(Phys.org)A trio of researchers with Columbia University has conducted a series of experiments regarding how much effort people are willing to exert in fact-checking news stories. In their paper published in Proceedings ...

Please sign in to add a comment. Registration is free, and takes less than a minute. Read more

More:
Improve evolution education by teaching genetics first - Phys.org - Phys.Org

Researchers help provide first glimpse of organelles in action inside living cells – Phys.Org

May 25, 2017 Researchers at Howard Hughes Medical Institute developed a way to produce color-tagged, 3D, microscopic videos of organelles in a live cell. They came to Drexel's Andrew Cohen, PhD, to develop an algorithm that could process massive amounts of visual data to better understand the behavior of organelles as a group and individually. This technology will help them unlock cell behavior and response to drug treatment. Credit: Drexel University

Researchers at Howard Hughes Medical Institute and the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute for Child Health and Human Development are getting a first glimpse at the inner-workings of live cells thanks to a new microscopy technique pioneered by Nobel laureate Eric Betzig with help from engineers at Drexel University. Their method uses grids of light that activate fluorescent color tags on each type of organellethe result is a 3-D video that gives researchers their best look at how cells function. It will allow scientists to better understand how cells react to environmental stressors and respond to drug treatment.

In a paper published today in Nature, the team lays out its methodology for using Betzig's lattice light sheet microscope in combination with image-tracking technology developed in Drexel's Computational Image Sequence Analysis Lab, led by Andrew Cohen, PhD, to produce 3-D time lapse videos of organelle movement and generate quantitative data on their interactions.

"The cell biology community has recognized for many years that the cytoplasm is full of many different types of organelles, and the field is recognizing more and more how significant cross-talk between these organelles is in the form of close contacts between these organelles," said Jennifer Lippincott-Schwartz, PhD, of HHMI's Janelia Research Campus, and senior author of the study. "When two organelles come close to each other they can transfer small molecules like lipids and calcium and communicate with each other through that transfer. But no one has been able to look at the whole set of these interactions at any particular time. This technology is providing a way to do that. But this paper is about a whole new technology, being able to tag six different objects with six different fluorophores, and unmixing the fluorophores so that you can observe the six different objects discretely."

Betzig's microscopy technique uses layers of light grids that interact with fluorescent protein-tagged cells to build a 3D microscopic image. At Janelia Research Campus, Betzig and Lippincott-Schwartz have refined that technology to produce a detailed look inside the cell by tagging each organelle type with its own color.

"The challenge is analyzing this data," Lippincott-Schwartz said. "It requires being able to simultaneously track these six different objects in 3D. What Andy Cohen and his group have done with the software system they have developed is enable us to really look at this in more quantitative ways than would be possible with conventional tools."

The video will load shortly

Cohen's lab developed a tool called LEVER 3-D in 2015 to help researchers study 3-D images of neural stem cells. It applies an advanced image segmentation algorithm they developed that can identify boundaries of cells and track their movements. Prior to this technology being available to microbiologists, the processing of microscopic images and time-lapse footage would take massive amounts of time because they would have to create lineage trees by hand and attempt to follow cell changes by making their own observations when comparing images.

This process is even more involved when multiple objects are being tracked in three dimensions. Lippincott-Schwartz's group used a battery of computer programs to filter out all the different pieces of light spectra emitted by the organelles, to begin to bring the 3-D images and video into focus. The process, called "linear unmixing," required more than 32 cores of a computer work station to sift through 7 billion sets of six-color images, pixel by pixel.

Typically they would use expensive commercial software programs to stitch them into a 3-D volume to go about studying them. But these programs are expensive and time-consuming to use, and were not capable of the sophisticated analysis for tracking moving objects in order to make quantitative measurements of their behaviors and particularly how they interact.

Cohen's algorithm automates the entire process, which saves researchers a lot of time and it also lets them ask and answer more questions about what the cells are doing. He further verified the data by working with Drexel colleague Uri Herschberg, PhD, an associate professor in the School of Biomedical Engineering, Science and Health Systems and College of Medicine, to check it against 2-D images of the cells.

"It's some really impressive footage that gives biologists this ability to look deeper and deeper into live cells and see things they've never seen beforelike six different organelles in a living cell in true 3-D," said Cohen, a professor in Drexel's College of Engineering. "But it's also a lot of work to begin quantifying what they're seeingand that's where we can help, by using our program to automate big portions of that process and glean valuable data from it."

The video will load shortly

Using the new technology to simultaneously look at six sets of organelles, Lippincott-Schwartz's teams at Janelia and at the National Institutes of Health are making exciting new observations. They are looking at how the organelles distribute themselves inside the cell, how often they interact with each other and where, when and how fast they move during various times in the cell's lifecycle.

"One very interesting outcome is that we found the largest organelle in the cell, which is the ER [endoplasmic reticulum], at any particular time point will be occupying about 25 percent of the volume of the cytoplasm, excluding the nucleus. But if you track the way it disperses through the cytoplasm over a short period of time, like 15 minutes, you see that it explores 95 percent of the whole cytoplasm during that time period," Lippincott-Schwartz said. "We can do this for all of the other organelles at the same time to see how the cytoplasm is being sensed through the dynamic motions of dispersive activities of these organelles."

Observing sub-cellular behavior is just the first application of this technology. Now that it has proven to generate usable data, the team will forge ahead to study what happens inside a cell when it is exposed to drug treatments and other common stresses on the system. The researchers suggest that it could be used to study many more than six types of microscopic objects. And it could help dig even deeper into the building blocks of lifeinto interactions of RNA particles and other proteins that play a role in a cell's function and the behavior of diseased cells.

"As these tools continue to improve they will give researchers both a better look at cell behavior and many options for gathering and analyzing that data," Cohen said. "They will be able to ask and answer increasingly complicated questions and that's going to lead to some very exciting and important discoveries."

Explore further: How plant cell compartments change with cell growth

More information: Alex M. Valm et al. Applying systems-level spectral imaging and analysis to reveal the organelle interactome, Nature (2017). DOI: 10.1038/nature22369

Journal reference: Nature

Provided by: Drexel University

A research team led by Kiminori Toyooka from the RIKEN Center for Sustainable Resource Science has developed a sophisticated microscopy technique that for the first time captures the detailed movement of subcellular organelles ...

(Phys.org)A team of researchers from the U.S. and the U.K. has used high-resolution imaging techniques to get a closer look at the endoplasmic reticulum (ET), a cellular organelle, and in so doing, has found that its structure ...

For hundreds of years biologists have studied cells through the lens of a microscope. With a little help from a team of engineers at Drexel University, these scientists could soon be donning 3-D glasses in a home-theater-like ...

The first analysis of how proteins are arranged in a cell was published today in Science, revealing that a large portion of human proteins can be found in more than one location in a given cell.

Scientists have made a breakthrough in understanding how different compartments (or organelles) of human cells interact.

For the first time, chemists have successfully produced an artificial cell containing organelles capable of carrying out the various steps of a chemical reaction. This was done at the Institute for Molecules and Materials ...

Mountain-dwelling East African honey bees have distinct genetic variations compared to their savannah relatives that likely help them to survive at high altitudes, report Martin Hasselmann of the University of Hohenheim, ...

A baby's babbles start to sound like speech more quickly if they get frequent vocal feedback from adults. Princeton University researchers have found the same type of feedback speeds the vocal development of infant marmoset ...

A team of researchers, led by a plant cell biologist at the University of California, Riverside, has for the first time identified a small RNA species and its target gene that together regulate female germline formation in ...

Researchers at the University of Iowa have discovered that a molecule which can sense the swelling of fat cells also controls a signaling pathway that allows fat cells to take up and store excess glucose. Mice missing this ...

Beekeepers across the United States lost 33 percent of their honey bee colonies during the year spanning April 2016 to April 2017, according to the latest preliminary results of an annual nationwide survey. Rates of both ...

Living cells must constantly process information to keep track of the changing world around them and arrive at an appropriate response.

Please sign in to add a comment. Registration is free, and takes less than a minute. Read more

Visit link:
Researchers help provide first glimpse of organelles in action inside living cells - Phys.Org

Biochemistry Analyzers Market: Global Growth by Manufacturers, Regions, Type and Application, Forecast Analysis to … – Green Mountain Outlook

Biochemistry Analyzers Marketreport provides key statistics on the market status of the Biochemistry Analyzers Manufacturers and is a valuable source of guidance and direction for companies and individuals interested in the Biochemistry Analyzers Industry. The Biochemistry Analyzers Market report delivers a basic overview of the industry including its definition, applications and manufacturing technology. Also, the Biochemistry Analyzers Industry report explores the international and Chinese Major Market players in detail. The Biochemistry Analyzers Market report presents the company profile, product specifications, capacity, production value, Contact Information of manufacturer and Biochemistry Analyzers Market shares for each company.

Get PDF Sample of Biochemistry Analyzers Market Report @ http://www.360marketupdates.com/enquiry/request-sample/10603758

Further in the report, Biochemistry Analyzers Market is examined for price, cost and revenue. In prolongation with this data sale price for various types, applications and region is also included.

Biochemistry Analyzers Market split by Product Type-Type 1, Type 2, Type3 Biochemistry Analyzers Market split by Application-Application 1, Application 2, Application 3 Biochemistry Analyzers Market Segment by RegionsUSA, EU, Japan, China and Others.

Other Major Topics Covered in Biochemistry Analyzers market report are as follows:

Manufacturing Technology of Biochemistry Analyzers Industry, Development of Biochemistry Analyzers, Manufacturing Technology, Analysis of Biochemistry Analyzers Manufacturing Technology, and Trends of Biochemistry Analyzers Manufacturing Technology, Analysis of Key Manufacturers of Biochemistry Analyzers Market, Company Profile, Product Information, Production Information, Contact Information, Global and Chinese Biochemistry Analyzers Market, Capacity, Production and Production Value of Biochemistry Analyzers Market, Global Cost and Profit of Biochemistry Analyzers Market, Market Comparison of Biochemistry Analyzers Industry, Supply and Consumption of Biochemistry Analyzers Market. Market Status of Biochemistry Analyzers Industry, Market Competition of Biochemistry Analyzers Industry by Company, Market Analysis of Biochemistry Analyzers Consumption by Application/Type and Region, Market Forecast of Global and Chinese Biochemistry Analyzers Market, Biochemistry Analyzers Market Cost and Profit Estimation, Global and Chinese Biochemistry Analyzers Market Share, Global and Chinese Supply and Consumption of Biochemistry Analyzers Market.

Inquire for further detailed information about Biochemistry Analyzers Market Report @ http://www.360marketupdates.com/enquiry/pre-order-enquiry/10603758

The Report explores detailed information about Market Dynamics of Biochemistry Analyzers Industry, Biochemistry Analyzers Industry News, Biochemistry Analyzers Industry Development Challenges, Biochemistry Analyzers Industry Development Opportunities, Proposals for New Project, Market Entry Strategies, Countermeasures of Economic Impact, Marketing Channels, Feasibility Studies of New Project Investment, Analysis of Biochemistry Analyzers Industry Chain, Industry Chain Structure, Upstream Raw Materials, Downstream Industry, Macroeconomic Outlook, Effects to Biochemistry Analyzers Industry.

In the end, the Biochemistry Analyzers Market report makes some important proposals for a new project of Biochemistry Analyzers Industry before evaluating its feasibility. Overall, the report provides an in-depth insight of 2012-2022 Global and Chinese Biochemistry Analyzers Market covering all important parameters.

See the original post:
Biochemistry Analyzers Market: Global Growth by Manufacturers, Regions, Type and Application, Forecast Analysis to ... - Green Mountain Outlook