Category Archives: Physiology

MCW: Physiology

The Department of Physiology at the Medical College of Wisconsin is dedicated to quality in research, graduate and postdoctoral training and medical education. The research interests of our faculty are broadly based, with strong programs in cardiovascular physiology, renal physiology, respiratory physiology, physiological genomics, proteomics, bioinformatics and computational biology. Read more about the Department of Physiology

The department of physiology would like to acknowledge and congratulate the following department members:

Shauna Rasmussen, 5th year graduate student in the lab of Dr. Aron Geurts, for winning the 26th Annual Graduate School Poster Session with her poster titled Utilization of immune compromised rats for allogeneic rat hepatocyte transplant model.

Dr. Justine Abais-Battad, postdoctoral fellow in the lab of Dr. Dave Mattson, for winning the 26th Annual Graduate School Poster Session with her poster titled Influence of Parental Dietary Protein Source on Dahl Salt-Sensitive Hypertension and Renal Disease.

Dr. Abais-Battad also received the $1,000 Edward J. Lennon, MD Award for an Outstanding Woman Postdoctoral Researcher for 2016.

Maria Angeles Baker, graduate student in Dr. Liang's lab, for successful defense of her thesis entitled miR-192-5p in the Kidney is Protective against the Development of Hypertension.

Three medical students who spent the summer conducting research in the labs of several physiology faculty received awards for their posters:

Original post:
MCW: Physiology

Introduction to physiology: History, biological systems …

Welcome to Medical News Today

Healthline Media, Inc. would like to process and share personal data (e.g., mobile ad id) and data about your use of our site (e.g., content interests) with our third party partners (see a current list) using cookies and similar automatic collection tools in order to a) personalize content and/or offers on our site or other sites, b) communicate with you upon request, and/or c) for additional reasons upon notice and, when applicable, with your consent.

Healthline Media, Inc. is based in and operates this site from the United States. Any data you provide will be primarily stored and processed in the United States, pursuant to the laws of the United States, which may provide lesser privacy protections than European Economic Area countries.

By clicking accept below, you acknowledge and grant your consent for these activities unless and until you withdraw your consent using our rights request form. Learn more in our Privacy Policy.

Read more:
Introduction to physiology: History, biological systems ...

Eye/Brain Physiology and Why Humans Don’t See Reality But …

="left"> Despite popular misconception, humans do not see a direct representation of external reality, but a translation formed by their eyes and mind. This is not some coffee house philosophical argument, but physiological fact. Human eyes do a good, but far from perfect job at detecting and processing light.

This page is an introuduction to the physiology of seeing and offers several interesting examples of optical distortion.

A Very Brief Overview of the Phsysiology of Seeing

When a human looks at an object, light from the object enters the eyes. The light goes through the cornea, which is a clear covering, then through the pupil which is a clear circle in the colored part of the eye called the iris. The pupil gets larger (dilates) when there is little light and smaller when there is little light. The lens focuses the light through the aqueous humor, a clear liquid, and onto the retina. The retina, in the back of the eye, contains millions of tiny photo sensors that detect the light. There are two main kinds of photo sensors: rods and cones. Shaped like rods, rods detect shades and forms and are needed for night and peripheral ('out of the corner of the eye') vision. Rods are not good at detecting color. Shaped like cones, cones are needed for seeing details, seeing in the bright daylight and seeing colors. Cones do not work well in low light, such as at night. Rods and cones cover the entire retina except for a spot where the optic nerve connects to the brain. The optic nerve carries the information received from the retina to the brain, where the brain translates it into the single image we perceive, or 'see.'

Blind Spots

All humans have blind spots, which are spots where the eye cannot see. The blind spot in an eye corresponds to the spot on the retina where the optical nerve connects the retina to the brain. At this spot there are no light detecting cells and, thus, this spot cannot detect light. A small object can disappear from view at the spot.

In everyday life the blind spot goes unnoticed. This is in part as the eye is constantly looking around, getting a wide and varied range of views. It is also in part as the brain uses the information from both eyes to create the single mental vision. What one eye misses, the other often picks up.

As its optical nerve connects differently, the octopus has no blind spot.

Detecting your blind spot

To detect your blind spot using the above red dot/green dot picture on the next page, close your right eye and look at the GREEN dot. Slowly move your head towards the picture. At one point the RED dot will disappear. Notice that the missing spot is filled in white by your mind, so it appears as if nothing is missing from your view. This illustrates how your blind spot goes unnoticed during daily living. Many people live their entire life not knowing they have a blind spot.

Humans have more glaring blind spots. Due to the placement of our eyes in our head, we can't naturally see behind us, under our feet, from the top of our head, behind our elbows. A common saying to explain why we didn't notice something is, "I don't have eyes in the back of my head." And it's common knowledge that if you want to sneak up on a person you approach from behind. We compensate for these blinds spots by turning around, moving our heads, using a mirror or other reflection, saying "Who's that behind me?," listening, noticing shadows.

Other animals have different eye placement and fields of view. As a robin has its eyes on the side of its head, it has better side view but worse directly ahead view. The robin's life depends on its being able to detecting predators from the side and back. When hunting for worms in the grass, robins turn their heads. Some think they are turning their ear to listen for worms, when they are turning their heads to see in front of them. A wolf, which is a hunter stalking prey, has eyes placement best suited for seeing ahead. The wolf sees better straight ahead, but its side to side vision is worse than a robin's. A crocodile has eyes that rise above the rest of its head. Not only does this create a different field of view, but allows the crocodile to see above water while the most of its head and body are hidden below water. The eyes serve as periscopes.

' Afterimages: Seeing What Isn't There

Afterimages are when, after staring at an object, you look away and still see an image of the object. An example is when you still see the nighttime headlights of a car, even when your eyes have closed and the car has turned away. Another is when, after looking away from a light bulb or candle in the dark, you still see light in the shape of the bulb or candle.

Afterimages happen after the retina's photosensors (the rods and cones in your eyes) become oversaturated, or burned out, from staring at a particular color. This burning out is comparable to lifting weights in the weight room. After doing enough bench presses you lose your bench press strength and will be able to lift only lighter weights. Your muscles are burnt out, if only temporarily, from lifting big weight. Similarly, after staring at a large area of a single color, the eye's photosensors lose their strength for that color. If, afterwards, the eyes look at a blank piece of paper, the photosensors will be weak towards the previously stared at color but fresh and strong for detecting the other colors. This imbalance causes the mind to perceive the image (the afterimage), but in the color opposite to the original color. To the mind, the weakness towards one color means the presence of the opposite primary color is stronger. Quirky perhaps, but this is the way the brain works. If you are staring at a green image, the afterimage should be red. After staring at a yellow image, the afterimage should be blue. The mind sees afterimages in primary colors, so any non-primary color will be seen as the primary opposite.

Though they occur almost constantly, afterimages usually go unnoticed. Afterimages are best observed when focusing on a single color or object for a lengthy of time. However, in normal viewing, we view a wide range of objects and colors at once and our eyes are always moving around, the view constantly shifting. In these cases, the afterimages are minor and get lost in the visual shuffle. We barely if at all notice them.

Natural delay in processing light

If in the dark you pass a lit match in front of your face you will see a trail of light following the match. If you pass you hand quickly in front of your eyes in daylight, your hand will be a blur. Related to afterimages, this effect happens in part because your eyes and brain don't process light instantaneously. It takes a small fraction of a second for the eyes and mind to translate the light that enters the eyes into the mental image we see in our minds.

This effect, along with the afterimage and binocular vision, aids in making our blind spot disappearing. As our eyes naturally move around, there is a lingering of image that helps cover the blind spot.

The following shows examples of afterimages, and a few also involving the process delay.

If you stare up close for about a minute at the below color squares, then stare at the corresponding white space below, you may perceive the colors in reverse.

If you stare at the below circular design, you should see movement of some sort, such as pulsating, shifting and/or rotating. This is is caused by how the eyes and mind detect and interep the information. As your eyes naturally move, even if slightly, an afterimage follows with your eyes causing the appearance of movement that does not exist. The rotating black and white design was intentionally designed to play on the afterimage and other visual conceits. To the human mind, if any printed picture is going to move on the page, it will be this circular, rotating design.

The below is another design that often produces the appearance of movement when stared at-such as rotating, pulsating and/or shifting. Even though the image is stationary, it's difficult to not visually perceive it as stationary.

Binocular Vision

Humans have binocular vision, meaning that the single image we see in our mind is made from two different views-- one from each eye.

Our binocular vision gives at least two notable advantages. First, we have a wider field of view than if we had only one eye. The right can see further to the right and the left further to the left. The single vision in our mind shows more than either single eye can see.

A second advantage is the two views give us good, if not perfect, depth perception. People who are blind in one eye and animals with only one eye have worse depth perception than the average human. The mythical Cyclops might appear an unbeatable warrior, but a wily human opponent could take advantage of the monster's poor depth perception.

Triangularism and Calculating Depth

Binocular vision produces the perception of depth in a way similar to how triangularism measures length in applied mathematics. When looking at a distant point using only one view it is hard to impossible to measure the distance accurately. In applied mathematics, triangularism can accurately calculate this distance from point a to point b by creating an imaginary triangle. Trianglularism has long been used in the real world to measure distant objects, like islands and boats at sea and when surveying land.

Triangularism: From point a alone, it can be impossible to accurately calculate distance to point b. In the real world, point a could be you standing on land and point b an anchored boat out at sea. However, by taking measurements from point a, then taking a measurement from nearby point c (perhaps a walking distance away), then measuring the distance from point a and c, one can create an imaginary triangle that calculates the distance from point a to point b. It's just a matter calculating angles and doing the math.

Two eyes give the mind a similar two point view of an apple or house, and the mind uses these two views to help guestimate distance. This is mostly done subconsciously. You simply reach out and grab that pencil or penny or door knob or hanging ceiling fan string or stairway railing, no problem. When you wear an eye patch, you may discover it's more difficult to grab things on the first try.

The Hole In The Hand Illusion

This simple trick plays with your binocular vision to make it appear as if you have a hole in your hand. Roll a normal piece of 8x11" paper into a tube and place it next to your hand as shown in the following picture. With one eye look through the tube and with the other at your hand. With a little bit of shifting you should perceive what appears to be a large hole through your hand. Your mind takes the two distinct views to create one odd bizarre view.

The viewer would look through the tube with his left eye and at his right hand with his right eye

As you can see, you don't see physical reality but a translation of it

When you are look at a living room or bowl of apples or painting or mountain range, the image you see is not a direct representation of the objects. The image you see is a translation made by your eyes and mind. As demonstrated, binocularism (changing two views into one), afterimages (images created by the eyes/mind), unnoticed blind spots, inability to see colors in low light and countless other purely physiological occurrences ensure that our mental image is always different than the objects viewed.

Everything we perceive involves visual illusion.

What color is a red ball when the lights are turned off? Remember that red is part of the visible light spectrum.

If you believe that there is a God who purposely created animals, why do you think He gave humans such limited eyesight? Why do you believe He gave some animals better eyesight than humans?

Infrared viewers, such as night vision goggles, do not allow humans to see infrared light, but translate infrared light into visible light. We will never see infrared light, and can only guess how an infrared viewing animal perceives the light.

Humans categorize and label objects in part by visible colors. Many animals, flowers, gems and even humans are defined by their colors.

As defined by the American Kennel Club, a cairn terrier can come in all colors except white. If a cairn terrier is born white, it's not a cairn terrier. It's a West Highland Terrier, a different breed.

If we could see infrared and ultraviolet light our categorizations and names of objects, including terriers, would be different.

A mirror mirrors what is in front of it. If you place an apple two feet in front of the mirror, an identical looking apple will look as if it's the same distance behind, or into, the mirror. Curiously, if you use triangulation to measure the distance to the apple in the mirror, the apple will measure as being two feet behind the mirror. Both our eyes and scientific measurement say there is an apple two feet behind the mirror's surface.

If a human perceives a person in a magazine picture and a dog does not, which animal has the better perception? Humans often use as evidence of a dog's dimwittedness that the dog 'doesn't see' the human being on the television screen, when, of course, there isn't really a person on the screen. The dog is faulted for not seeing what isn't there.

Other Senses

Smell, taste, sound and touch effect your visual perception. For example, your visual perception of a pie shaped object may be confirmed, corrected or confused by the smell. You judge distance by sound-something is usually softer the further away it gets. In the dark, people typically feel about for walls, doors and tables. Echoes can fool you into misjudging location.

While humans depend mostly on sight, other animals depend more on other senses. The blood hound has worse than human eyesight, but uses its advanced sense of smell to find lost people that even trained police detectives cannot find. In these instances, the blood hound's non-seeing perception is more accurate than all of the detectives senses combined. This explains why many police departments have blood hounds on the staff.

Read more:
Eye/Brain Physiology and Why Humans Don't See Reality But ...

Nobel in physiology, medicine awarded to three Americans for …

Three Americans Jeffrey C. Hall, Michael Rosbash and Michael W. Young won the 2017 Nobel Prize in physiology for shedding light on how an "inner clock" can fluctuate to optimize human behavior and physiology. (Reuters)

Three Americans Jeffrey C. Hall, Michael Rosbash and Michael W. Young have won the 2017 Nobel Prize in physiology or medicine for their discoveries about themechanisms that control an organism's circadian responses to light and dark.

Hall was born in New York, Rosbash in KansasCity, and they both worked at Brandeis University when they began their Nobel-winning work. Hall is now affiliated with the University of Maine. Michael Young was born in Miami and works at Rockefeller University.

In announcing the winner in Stockholm on Monday, the prize committee said thescientists elucidated howa life-form'sinner clock can fluctuate to optimize our behavior and physiology.Their discoveries explain how plants, animals and humans adapt their biological rhythm so that it is synchronized with the Earth's revolutions.

Working with fruit flies, thescientists isolated a gene that is responsible for a protein that accumulates in the night but is degraded in the day.Misalignments in this clock may play a rolein medical conditions and disorders, as well as thetemporary disorientationofjet lag that travelers experience when crisscrossing time zones.

The circadian system has its tentacles around everything, Rosbash, a Howard Hughes Medical Institute investigator, explainedin the HHMI Bulletinin 2014. Its ticking away in almost every tissue in the human body. It's also in plants, including major food crops, the article noted, and appears to be tiedto disease susceptibility, growthrate, and fruit size.

Young said that in the early days of their researchmanyscientists thought of their work as a subset of neuroscience. They theorizedthat the brain may have a single, central clock controlling the cycles we've observed such asthe rise and fall of our body temperature and blood pressure throughout the day.Now we know each living thing, includingthose without brains, may havemanydifferentclocks.

We learned we are truly rhythmic organisms, Youngsaid in a phone interview. Today, its hard to find a cellthat does not oscillatein response to these clocks.

From left to right,Michael Rosbash, Jeffrey C. Hall, and Michael W. Young. (AFP PHOTO/CHINESE UNIVERSITY OF HONG KONG)

The trio's early work took place in 1984 when Hall and Rosbash worked together at Brandeis and Young at Rockefeller University to isolate the period gene, which controls the circadian rhythm of fruit flies. Hall and Rosbash then showed that the level of the protein encoded by this gene changes in a 24-hour cycle, going up during the day and down at night. They theorized that this protein blocked the activity of the period gene.

But to have this effect, the protein would have to reach the genetic material in the cell nucleus, and no one was able to figure out how it got there until Young, in 1994, discovered a second clock gene, now known as timeless. He showed that when the protein encoded by timeless bound to the protein made by the gene period, they were able to enter the cell nucleus. He further identified a third gene, doubletime, which appeared to control the frequency of the oscillations over a 24-hour period.

Erin OShea, president of the Howard Hughes Medical Institute, said that people have observed for centuries that plants and animals change their behavior in sync with the light present in the natural environment. What Hall, Rosbash and Young figured out is how this happens.

Genes make up the mechanics by which organisms can keep track of time and this allows them just like your wristwatch to coordinate their behavior their sleep-wake cycle with the changes in the light-dark cycle, she said.

The circadian clock anticipates and adapts our physiology to the different phases of the day. (The Nobel Assembly at Karolinska Institutet)

Researchers in the field of circadian biology or chronobiology, as it is nicknamed said Monday that the scientists work has had a major influence on their work in humanhealth and medicine.Alzheimers, depression, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), heart disease, obesity and diabetes and other metabolic issues are among the many conditions that appear to be linked to circadian rhythms being out of whack.

Erol Fikrig, a researcher atYale University who is studyingwhether the timing of insect bites impacts our abilityto fight off diseases like dengue fever or Lyme disease, explained that our immune system, too, is influenced by circadian rhythm, which can alter our ability to fight infections.

Amita Sehgal, a neuroscientist at the University of Pennsylvania, was a postdoctoral student in Youngs lab from 1988 to 1993 and worked on the clock genes. Herresearch these days involves looking at how sleep appears to be controlled by the circadian clock. Although we sleep at night, our need to sleep appears to be independent of the clock. If you didn't have a clock, you would still sleep, but it would be randomly distributed throughout the day, she said.

Young said that one of the most important areas of study built on their work is what happens when the clock runs too fast or too slow. Most recently, scientists have discovered that one percent of humans worldwide have a mutation in the clock genes that is associated with delayed sleep or being a night owl. He said many of these individuals show up at sleep clinics wondering what to do, andthe work provides a target to work on.

Thats powerful information that can inform lots of future work in the development of therapies,he said.

Young said there's also growing research mostly in animals that supports the idea thatmaintaining a more regularschedule, including eating and sleeping, may contribute to longevity.

This may be back to going back to what people used to do before they had refrigerators and electric lights, he said.

This year's winners probably weren't in a lot of Nobel Prize betting pools, because the medicine Nobel is notoriously hard to predict. In fact, during a news conference at which the awards were announced, a member of theNobel Assembly at Karolinska Institutet said that when heinformed Rosbash that he had received the award, his response was, You are kidding me.

Thehighly secretive Nobel committeedoes not release a list those under consideration for its awards and never has in its 116-year history. The names being thrownaround as deserving of the prize in the weeks before the announcement is always very long and highly speculative. David Pendlebury, formerly of Thomson Reuters and now with Clarivate Analytics, bases his picks on an extensive data analysis and has an impressive track record of correctlypicking numerousNobel Laureates over the past 15 years (although not always in the right year).

This year, heidentified as possible winners Yuan Chang and Patrick Moore of the University of Pittsburgh for their work with human herpesvirus 8 (KSHV/HHV8) which is associated with cancer; Lewis Cantley of Weill Cornell of Medicine forthe discovery of a cellsignalingpathway and its role in tumor growth; andKarl J. Friston of the University College London for his work on algorithms and techniques for the analysis of brain imaging data. Perhaps one of those picks will win next year.

Inrecentyears, the Nobel in medicine hasbeen awarded for breakthroughs in a wide range of work in human biology: a Japanese scientist who discovered a key mechanism in our bodys defense system that involves recycling parts of cells and plays an important role in cancer; a trio who worked on treatments for river blindness and malaria; and researchers whodeciphered thebrain's GPS thatallows us to orient ourselves in space.

The Nobel Prize in physics will be announced on Tuesday, the chemistry award on Wednesday, literature on Thursday and the peace prize on Friday. An award in economics in memory of Alfred Nobel (which is not one of the original Nobel Prizes) will be announced Monday.

Read more:

Sleep study on modern-day hunter-gatherers dispels notion that were wired to need 8 hours a day

What a year of working the graveyard shift taught me about sleep

Rejoice, weekend warriors: a few days of camping tunes up your circadian clock

Scientists offer help to labs hit by Harvey, even tending to their fruit flies

Could some ADHD be a type of sleep disorder? That would fundamentally change how we treat it.

2016:Nobel Prize in medicine awarded to Japans Yoshinori Ohsumi for work on cell recycling

Quiz: How much do you know about the Nobel Peace Prize?

Read the original here:
Nobel in physiology, medicine awarded to three Americans for ...

ARTICLES | Physiology

Abstract

Paralysis due to spinal cord injury can severely limit motor function and independence. This review summarizes different approaches to electrical stimulation of the spinal cord designed to restore motor function, with a brief discussion of their origins and the current understanding of their mechanisms of action. Spinal stimulation leads to impressive improvements in motor function along with some benefits to autonomic functions such as bladder control. Nonetheless, the precise mechanisms underlying these improvements and the optimal spinal stimulation approaches for restoration of motor function are largely unknown. Finally, spinal stimulation may augment other therapies that address the molecular and cellular environment of the injured spinal cord. The fact that several stimulation approaches are now leading to substantial and durable improvements in function following spinal cord injury provides a new perspectives on the previously incurable condition of paralysis.

Paralysis due to spinal cord injury affects ~282,000 people in the U.S. (70). Spinal cord injury can lead to paralysis of both the upper and lower extremities, severely limiting activities of daily living. People with tetraplegia (paralysis of upper and lower limbs) cite restoration of hand and arm function as their highest priority for functional recovery (2, 22). People with paraplegia (paralysis of only the lower limbs) cite walking movement as a priority, although notably a lower priority than restoration of autonomic functions (2). Although many types of therapy may be prescribed for spinal cord injury rehabilitation, only a few of the most commonly applied methods have demonstrated reliable effects (37), and most do not completely restore motor function of the paralyzed limbs.

The application of electrical stimulation to treat spinal cord injury has garnered substantial interest from the research community, since it can enhance the electrical activity of neurons after spinal cord injury and may help to restore function. The spinal cord is an attractive target for stimulation-based rehabilitative therapies, since interventions at the spinal level can take advantage of preserved motor and sensory neural pathways below the injury. Stimulation of the spinal cord allows fatigue-resistant movements (42), which are typically difficult to achieve with more distal stimulation sites, such as peripheral nerves or muscles (53, 66). Spinal stimulation can also produce complex movements involving multiple muscles and joints, such as those required for walking (41, 63), reaching, and grasping (60, 90, 97).

Several groups have used electrical stimulation to improve limb function in awake, behaving, paralyzed animals. This includes the production of hindlimb stepping movements in paralyzed rats (8, 85), cats (5, 42, 63), and non-human primates (11), as well as forelimb reaching movements in rats (49) and non-human primates (60, 69, 97).

In addition to its somatic effects, spinal stimulation may also benefit the autonomic nervous system. Spinal stimulation during motor training can improve and trigger bladder voiding in animal models (26, 73), and a human case study cited improvements in bladder control, sexual function, and temperature regulation after motor training with spinal stimulation (36). Although this review is primarily focused on somatic motor improvements, these broad benefits illustrate that stimulation focused on motor rehabilitation may also confer important autonomic benefits to people with spinal cord injury (2).

Furthermore, targeted electrical stimulation may be useful in directing the rehabilitation of specific motor pathways (21). The spinal cord undergoes neural remodeling after injury, and this remodeling can lead to maladaptive changes in neural pathways that may increase detrimental effects such as pain and spasticity (6, 48). Targeted therapeutic electrical stimulation may guide these remodeling mechanisms toward the formation of functional, rather than maladaptive, neural pathways.

Researchers have identified several methods of spinal cord stimulation for the restoration of movement after paralyzing spinal cord injury. Stimulation can be delivered 1) epidurally, with electrodes on the dorsal surface of the cord above the dura; 2), transcutaneously, with electrodes placed on the skin above the vertebral column; and 3) intraspinally, with stimulating electrodes implanted within the spinal cord (FIGURE 1). The site of stimulation partly determines the neural pathways activated as well as the stimulation parameters required to elicit the desired result (86). The different approaches to spinal stimulation may also differ in their mechanisms of action; current opinion in the field is that intraspinal stimulation likely activates motor pools as well as intraspinal and propriospinal networks to enable coordinated whole-limb movements (91), whereas sub-threshold epidural and transcutaneous stimulation may increase the baseline excitability of the spinal cord, thereby enabling movements triggered by inputs that remain intact after spinal cord injury (20).

Illustrations of the location of epidural, intraspinal, and transcutaneous spinal stimulation

Left: illustrations of the location of epidural stimulation compared with intraspinal microstimulation, both applied distal to a contusion injury. Right: stimulation location and stimulation parameters for transcutaneous stimulation applied to the cervical spinal cord utilizing a 10-kHz carrier frequency to improve hand function after spinal cord injury.

Although electrical stimulation treatments for various disorders can be traced back as early as the first century, the utility of spinal cord stimulation for the restoration of motor function is a more recent development. Therapeutic electrical stimulation of the spinal cord first emerged as a treatment for pain described in clinical cases in the late 1960s and early 1970s (17, 87, 88). In a 1967 experiment, Shealy and colleagues stimulated the spinal cord through a single electrode placed on the dura of a man complaining of diffuse chest and abdominal pain. Stimulating at frequencies of 1050 Hz caused a buzzing sensation for the patient but also eliminated his pain for 515 min, after which a change in stimulation frequency was required to continue control of the pain (88). This stimulation was believed to inhibit the conduction of pain signals via activating larger sensory fibers in the dorsal columns of the spinal cord and quieting the smaller pain fibers (87). Later experimental results demonstrated effective pain relief using bipolar electrode arrangements, which allowed for higher-frequency (100200 Hz) stimulation while remaining well below the tissue damage threshold (7.75103 W/cm2) (87).

The clinical applications of spinal cord stimulation quickly expanded, as other groups observed its benefits for improved motor and sensory function in people with multiple sclerosis. Using similar epidural stimulation implants, Cook and Weinstein reported improvements in spasticity, motor function, and sensory function in people with multiple sclerosis treated with epidural spinal stimulation (12). Less than a decade later, Campos and colleagues reported improved motor function and bladder control, among other positive effects, following therapeutic spinal stimulation in people with spinal cord injury as well as in those with multiple sclerosis (10). Interestingly, Dimitrijevic and colleagues later observed variable effects of electrical stimulation on relief of spasticity in people with spinal cord injury, citing differences in body position (e.g., standing vs. sitting), differences in spasticity at a given time, and patient-controlled stimulation strength as likely contributors to this variability (18). The same group later outlined standards for epidural stimulation to relieve spasticity, observing that 50- to 100-Hz stimulation of 2- to 7-V strength and 210-ms pulse width worked best. They also noted, however, that the stimulation could be further optimized for each patient by testing different electrode combinations and adjusting stimulus amplitude based on body position (74), highlighting the anatomical variability of spinal pathways similar to those seen between the brains of different people.

Several groups proceeded to refine electrical stimulation of the spinal cord in animal models. They tested the threshold level of stimulation required to evoke movements, then experimented with stimulation that directly evoked movements (supra-threshold). The Skinner group demonstrated that supra-threshold stimulation of the dorsal surface of the spinal cord at a frequency of 35 Hz could reliably elicit stepping movements in decerebrated cats (44), and the Edgerton group induced bilateral stepping movements via similar methods in rats, observing that bilateral stepping movements occurred most often with 40- to 50-Hz stimulation delivered over the second lumbar spinal segment (43). More recently, the Courtine group observed improved locomotion with supra-threshold epidural stimulation in non-human primates with spinal cord injuries (11).

In contrast to supra-threshold stimulation, sub-threshold stimulation does not immediately evoke movements but may permit the animal to execute movements in contexts such as treadmill locomotion. For example, the Edgerton group demonstrated that sub-threshold stimulation could induce movement in the presence of proprioceptive inputs, likely by amplifying existing reflexive activity in the rat spinal cord (23).

Parallel findings were observed in human participants. Dimitrijevics group demonstrated that supra-threshold epidural stimulation at 515 Hz could result in lower limb extension in people with complete spinal cord injury and paraplegia. They hypothesized that this effect relied on the activation of primary sensory afferents, which in turn activated a network of neurons within the spinal cord to elicit motor unit activity and muscle contraction (47). Human and animal studies indicate that epidural stimulation of the spinal cord may induce movements either by direct electrical activation of motor or sensory units (43, 44, 47) or by the facilitation, or increase, of baseline motor unit activity (23, 30). Increasing baseline motor unit activity could bring the motor units closer to threshold, the level of activity required to produce a movement. This sub-threshold stimulation technique has garnered substantial interest in the spinal stimulation research community in recent years.

In contrast to direct activation of motor units, epidural stimulation more recently enabled otherwise paralyzed people to make volitional movements in the presence of continuous, sub-threshold stimulation. Recent work has demonstrated the utility of this enabling epidural stimulation for promoting both lower (3, 36) and upper (52) limb movements. These studies show an impressive return of voluntary lower limb movement with epidural stimulation in people with complete and incomplete spinal cord injuries (3, 36), as well as improved volitional hand control with epidural stimulation in people with motor-limiting cervical spinal cord injuries (52).

For some participants, the benefits of sub-threshold epidural stimulation persist beyond the period of stimulation. The exciting therapeutic benefit was noted by both participants in the upper-extremity study, whose hand function remained improved after stimulation had ceased (52). This encouraging result further supports the need for an evaluation of the circuits activated by epidural stimulation. Just as customized stimulation parameters such as stimulation frequency and amplitude for individual patients and specific tasks typically lead to better functional improvements during the stimulation period (1, 76, 77, 85), customized parameters and pairing with other rehabilitation strategies geared toward enabling sustained function after stimulation may provide an additional benefit.

The mechanisms responsible for the effect of epidural stimulation on paralyzed limbs have intrigued many researchers. A 1975 review of early work in neural stimulation described the multitude of factors at play, explaining that an understanding of the precise cells and tissues activated by stimulation will require detailed knowledge of the cell and tissue properties, electrode configurations, and stimulus parameters, such as waveform shape, duration, and magnitude (75). Although the field may yet lack a complete understanding of the complex interactions of these elements, some generalized mechanistic explanations have emerged, as described below.

Although epidural stimulation activates both afferent and efferent pathways (57, 79), supraspinal and sensory inputs driven by the patients intent and position may dictate the specific motor units recruited for a given task. For example, it is likely that the sensory signals produced by weight-bearing standing can selectively enhance the activity of relevant motor units during stimulation periods (34, 79), effectively increasing activity in the units required to maintain an upright position. Supraspinal input in the form of volitional, conscious motor commands can control lower limb movement in a supine position in the presence of epidural stimulation, even for people with clinically motor and sensory complete injuries (3, 34, 36). These results are aligned with the current general view of epidural stimulation as an enabling technology capable of enhancing baseline activity or physiological state of the spinal cord such that additional inputs such as proprioceptive inputs in the case of locomotion may activate the appropriate motor pathways for a given task (20). In one study, all 12 participants with motor complete spinal cord injuries could voluntarily produce electromyographic activity in two independent muscles of the paralyzed limbs, although not sufficient to result in movement (61). This provides evidence of spared pathways passing the spinal cord injury in nearly all persons with clinically complete injuries. These spared pathways may carry signals that could be useful in triggering movements in the presence of spinal stimulation. In most cases, epidural stimulation must be applied to observe benefits (3, 34, 76), and carryover of lower extremity motor benefits after the period of stimulation is limited (76). Nonetheless, benefits to autonomic functions such as bladder, bowel, and sexual function persist beyond the period of stimulation in both human (36) and animal subjects (26), suggesting a persistent and beneficial reorganization of spinal neural pathways is possible due to stimulation therapy.

Both electrical and magnetic stimulation applied to the skin surface can improve motor function after injury. Magnetic stimulation applied over the lumbar spinal cord improves spasticity for up to 24 h following stimulation (50). Similarly, transcutaneous electrical stimulation applied over the thoracic spinal processes leads to improvement in spasticity and augmented stepping ability during stimulation periods for people with spinal cord injuries (39, 40, 55). The adoption of high-frequency electrical stimulation permits the application of higher current transcutaneous stimulation to the skin above the spinal cord with minimal discomfort (93, 94). The 10-kHz carrier frequency (FIGURE 1, RIGHT) permits over 100 mA of current to pass through the skin without painful sensations (27). Such stimulation is capable of activating the lumbar spinal cord both in spinally intact (28, 31, 80, 81) and injured participants (24, 25, 27, 29). As with epidural stimulation, the effects of transcutaneous stimulation depend on body position. For example, the current required to elicit a movement is greater in prone compared with standing positions, and the magnitude of the response is highest in supine compared with standing and prone positions (16). These findings reinforce the context-dependent nature of spinal neural pathways that may be leveraged for therapy.

Several groups are also exploring transcutaneous spinal stimulation applied to the cervical region for improving hand and arm function with promising results. Early results suggest that transcutaneous stimulation may confer similar benefits to those of epidural stimulation (29).

Although the electrodes are positioned further from the spinal cord, the basic mechanisms responsible for the effects of transcutaneous stimulation likely also rely on increasing baseline electrical activity to enable movements induced by remaining volitional motor commands or sensory inputs. The Gerasimenko group recently demonstrated that specific electrode configurations can also contribute to enhanced effects of transcutaneous spinal stimulation (81). By stimulating at two sites in rostro-caudal order, first at the site closer to the head and then at the site further down the spinal cord, they were able to elicit stronger responses than by stimulating at individual locations. They suggest that this may be explained by recruitment of motoneurons via both direct and indirect (e.g., sensory, interneuron) pathways in the rostro-caudal stimulation paradigm (81).

Because transcutaneous approaches do not require surgery, they may be more attractive to some people. This experimental approach, however, is quite new, and optimal application schedules and activities to be performed during stimulation are still being discovered. Although precise parameters used in epidural stimulation are unlikely to translate given the more distant application of current through the skin, a common theme appears to be the need for intensive therapy and exercise to be performed during the application of spinal cord stimulation to realize the full benefits to motor function (3, 24).

Intraspinal stimulation differs from epidural and transcutaneous stimulation in that it delivers electrical current through electrodes implanted within the spinal cord. Thus far, intraspinal stimulation studies in humans are rare, but animal work provides insights into the potential benefits of this approach. Intraspinal stimulation can elicit a wide variety of functionally relevant movements in animal models, including movements required for stepping (33, 42, 51, 6367, 78). It can also elicit a variety of movements related to reaching and grasping (60, 90, 97). When intraspinal stimulation is applied to the ventral spinal cord, direct activation of motoneurons or ventral root axons can occur, leading to single joint movement. When it is applied to the intermediate lamina of the spinal cord, consensus is that stimulation most likely activates axons and subsequently interneurons, cells within the spinal cord that can in turn activate complex neural pathways and result in coordinated motor patterns. This is because electrical stimulation generally activates fibers of passage rather than cell bodies (75), and interneuron fibers are abundant within the spinal cord. The activated interneurons can then activate the reflex and movement coordination pathways in which they participate, which may lead to coordinated multi-joint movements.

Intraspinal stimulation may be especially useful when paired with physical rehabilitation in experiments geared toward activating specific motor pathways or strengthening synapsesthe connections between neurons. Rodent studies have demonstrated lasting forelimb motor improvements after intraspinal stimulation of a specific movement (49, 54), even weeks after stimulation had ceased (54, 56). These results allude to the potential long-term therapeutic effect of intraspinal stimulation. Pairing intraspinal stimulation with rehabilitative physical training may have added benefits, and specific studies that directly address a combined approach would be useful.

Intraspinal stimulation may also confer more benefits if the user can easily control the stimulation. One way to enable such control would be to use signals that are already present during a particular task, such as a brain signal that occurs when an animal attempts to move. Stimulation controlled by activity-related signals is called activity-dependent stimulation. Activity-dependent stimulation may confer long-term benefits when the time between recording of the activity signal and delivery of stimulation falls within a specific time window (54). Activity-dependent stimulation that takes advantage of this time window can strengthen cortico-cortical (45) and cortico-spinal (68) connections in uninjured animals, and further investigation of the utility of this approach in spinal cord injury rehabilitation is warranted. It would be extremely useful to understand the maximum duration of these changes and whether they can be extended from days (68) and weeks (54) to months or years.

Although the stimulation of the spinal cord at epidural, transcutaneous, and intraspinal locations as described above has led to substantial advancements in the field, all of these approaches currently fall short of fully restoring natural movements and achieving long-term rehabilitation. Our understanding of the underlying mechanisms responsible for the effect of exogenous stimulation on biological tissue is incomplete, leading to challenges in translation from animal models to humans (13) and difficulty facilitating motor improvements that persist beyond the period of stimulation. Additionally, although epidural stimulation benefits from widespread clinical acceptance due to its long history as a pain treatment, translation of intraspinal stimulation methods will likely take more time, since development of hardware and novel surgical and application techniques is still underway.

Nonetheless, electrical stimulation shows therapeutic potential in the treatment of spinal cord injury motor deficits, and investigations of the underlying mechanisms and optimal stimulation parameters should continue to drive progress toward restoring natural movements to paralyzed limbs.

Although promising early results of electrical spinal stimulation indicate a prominent role in enhancing motor recovery, the potential of this technology to elicit long-term, sustained improvements will most likely require further refinement and perhaps a combination of multiple treatment approaches. Such approaches are referred to as combinatorial treatments, since they include a combination of interventions.

Such combinatorial interventions might target the molecular environment of the injured spinal cord to further increase its excitability and enhance the effects of therapeutic electrical stimulation. For example, pharmacological agents that increase excitability, such as serotonergic agonists or inhibitory neurotransmitter antagonists, appear to enhance the effects of epidural stimulation (8, 32). These pharmacological treatments can in some cases enhance the effects of locomotor training (21) and epidural stimulation (14). When administered orally during a period of transcutaneous stimulation treatment in humans, the serotonergic agonist buspirone enhanced motor function during and beyond acute stimulation treatments (29). This is consistent with prior results in animals, in which quipazine, another serotonergic agonist, appeared to regulate the stepping rhythm induced by epidural stimulation (30). Combinatorial approaches that employ multiple tools may prove to be the most useful. For example, combinations of low-dose pharmacological agents, electrical stimulation, and motor training have demonstrated functional improvements in animal models of spinal cord injury (8, 19, 21). The combinations of stimulation and pharmacological approaches are reviewed in greater detail elsewhere (21, 30).

In addition to pharmacological agents that directly affect spinal cord excitability, agents that enhance the plasticity of the spinal cord may also improve motor outcomes. These agents typically interfere with molecular pathways that inhibit plasticity. For example, approaches that interfere with Nogo-A, a component of myelin that inhibits neurite outgrowth, can enhance cerebrospinal tract sprouting and improve hindlimb locomotion after spinal cord injury (84). Similarly, dissolution of chondroitin sulfate proteoglycans (CSPGs), extracellular matrix components that limit synapse formation (7), has resulted in sprouting of ascending and descending neural projections and improved motor function (7, 46, 89). Interestingly, a combination of anti-Nogo and enzymatic treatment to dissolve CSPGs yielded greater improvements in motor function than either treatment alone (96). Ongoing and future work testing the combination of electrical spinal stimulation and these plasticity-promoting interventions is a promising avenue to improve function after spinal cord injury.

Although pharmacological interventions may promote excitability and plasticity in the cells that remain viable after spinal cord injury, replacing damaged cells may further enhance electrical stimulation treatments. Spinal cord injury often results in cellular damage and demyelination or dysmyelination, whereby the insulating material that enables efficient electrical conduction through axons is lost or damaged. Stem cells might promote the repair of this damage; for example, neural and glial cells derived from transplanted neural stem and progenitor cells (62, 71, 92) promote remyelination of axons near the injury site and promote motor improvement after spinal cord injury (15, 38). However, because stem cells may mature into many different cell types, it is important to carefully direct the cell toward a specific type, or fate, before transplant to achieve optimal results (38). Stem cell and neural progenitor treatments also improve reaching performance and hand function (72, 83).

Intriguingly, stem cells might also respond to therapeutic electrical stimulation, potentially by migrating toward the site of injury and providing support to damaged neurons or by forming new neural networks to bridge the gaps caused by spinal cord injury. Early results from studies investigating this combination are promising. Electrical stimulation appears to promote transplanted cell survival after peripheral nerve axotomy in vivo (35), indicating the potential for a positive interaction of the two approaches. Additionally, the application of electrical current can affect neural stem cell migration in vitro (4, 58, 95). Based on these findings, perhaps electrical stimulation could be used to guide stem cells toward sites of cellular damage in vivo.

Taken together, these currently disparate approaches suggest many potential avenues for combined therapeutic electrical stimulation, cell-based, and pharmacological therapies in future work. The early successes of electrical stimulation therapies are encouraging, but restoring complete function may require the combination of many approaches that address the multi-faceted effects of spinal cord injury. It is an exciting time in the field of spinalcord injury, since future studies have a multitude of potentially viable treatment options to explore. Going forward, careful and systematic evaluation of therapeutic stimulation approaches and their combinations with molecular and cellular interventions may be necessary to deliver effective new treatments to benefit people with spinal cord injuries.

This research was supported by the NSF Graduate Research Fellowship Program (GRFP), the Center for Sensorimotor Neural Engineering (CSNE), a National Engineering Research Center (EEC-1028725), a Paul G. Allen Family Foundation Allen Distinguished Investigator Award, the Christopher and Dana Reeve Foundation International Consortium on Spinal Cord Repair, The Craig H. Neilsen Foundation SCIRTS program (no. 259314), the U.S. DOD/CDMRP SCIRP (SC120209), and the GSK/Galvani Innovation Challenge Fund.

No conflicts of interest, financial or otherwise, are declared by the author(s).

Author contributions: A.I. drafted manuscript; A.I. and C.T.M. edited and revised manuscript; A.I. and C.T.M. approved final version of manuscript.

Read more:
ARTICLES | Physiology

Introduction to Physiology: History And Scope

This page was printed from: https://www.medicalnewstoday.com/articles/248791.php

Visit http://www.medicalnewstoday.com for medical news and health news headlines posted throughout the day, every day.

2017 Healthline Media UK Ltd. All rights reserved. MNT is the registered trade mark of Healthline Media. Any medical information published on this website is not intended as a substitute for informed medical advice and you should not take any action before consulting with a healthcare professional.

Read more:
Introduction to Physiology: History And Scope

New generation drugs may hold key to alternative erectile dysfunction treatment – Medical Xpress

Close to 70 percent of men with erectile dysfunction (ED) respond to the ED drug sildenafil. However, only about 50 percent of men with diabetesa population commonly affected by EDachieve positive results with sildenafil. Researchers from the Smooth Muscle Research Centre at the Dundalk Institute of Technology, in Dundalk, Ireland, are studying two new drugs that may give men with diabetesand others for whom conventional treatment is ineffectivenew hope for treating ED. The article is published ahead of print in the American Journal of PhysiologyCell Physiology.

For a successful erection to occur, the smooth muscle in the erectile tissue (corpus cavernosum) must relax, explained Keith Thornbury, PhD, corresponding author of the study. However, constant erection is prevented because the tissue contracts. The contraction is controlled by negatively charged chloride moving from inside the cells to outside. In a chain reaction, the outward flow of chloride causes calcium to flow into the cells. When calcium enters the smooth muscle cells of the erectile tissue, it contracts, preventing erection.

The research team treated cells from the corpus cavernosum of male rabbits with two different chloride channel blocking drugs. They found that both drugs were able to block the flow of chloride, causing the smooth muscle cells to relax. This is a different pathway to treating ED than sildenafil, which hinders the effects of a blood flow-regulating enzyme. "This suggests that chloride channels play an important role in maintaining detumescence [state of non-erection] in the corpus cavernosum and, therefore, might present a future target for treating erectile dysfunction," the researchers wrote.

Explore further: What causes erectile dysfunction and should it be checked?

More information: Karen I Hannigan et al. The role of Ca 2+ -activated Cl - current in tone generation in the rabbit corpus cavernosum, American Journal of Physiology - Cell Physiology (2017). DOI: 10.1152/ajpcell.00025.2017

Follow this link:
New generation drugs may hold key to alternative erectile dysfunction treatment - Medical Xpress

SVS hosts 1st natl conference on Physiology of Ageing – The Hans India

Mahbubnagar: The SVS Medical College hosted ATPCON-2017the first national conference on physiology of ageing on its campus in Mahbubnagar. The conference was organised by the Telangana Association of Physiologists for two days.

Dr K Krishna Reddy, Secretary of SVS Education Society, who presided over as chief guest, inaugurated the two-day event which was held on August 30 and 31.

More than 300 delegates from Andhra Pradesh, Tamil Nadu, Kerala and Karnataka participated in the event. Eminent Physiologists gave their presentations on the latest research developments in the subject of ageing. The event witnessed 61 paper presentations from eminent research scholars.

Dr KJ Reddy, Medical Director of SVS, while emphasising the need for dissemination of latest information on all aspects of ageing, including methods to be adapted in lifestyles for improving the quality of life, also gave a presentation on osteoarthritis, the most important issue affecting the elderly.

Dr Mohd Abrar, President ATP, Dr Narsinga Rao, Dr Rameshwari Reddy Dr BA Rama Rao and Dr Joshi participated in the inauguration. Senior eminent Physiologists Dr Jaya Vikrama Reddy, Dr Keshava Rao and Dr Kumudini Mohan Rao were felicitated on the day.

Read the rest here:
SVS hosts 1st natl conference on Physiology of Ageing - The Hans India

Watch your neck: Physiology and the advent of the smartphone … – Hawaii Tribune Herald

Comments posted below are from readers. In no way do they represent the view of Oahu Publishing Inc. or this newspaper.This is a public forum.

Comments may be monitored for inappropriate content but the newspaper is under no obligation to do so. Comment posters are solely responsible under the Communications Decency Act for comments posted on this Web site. Oahu Publishing Inc. is not liable for messages from third parties.

IP and email addresses of persons who post are not treated as confidential records and will be disclosed in response to valid legal process.

Do not post:

If you believe that a commenter has not followed these guidelines, please click the FLAG icon below the comment.

More here:
Watch your neck: Physiology and the advent of the smartphone ... - Hawaii Tribune Herald

Another Viewpoint: Watch your neck: Physiology and the advent of the smartphone – News Chief

In his 2010 book "I Live in the Future & Here's How It Works," the technology writer Nick Bilton relayed anecdotes about early 19th-century anxieties in Britain at the dawn of train travel.

It was thought that "people would asphyxiate if carried at speeds of more than 20 mph" and reputable scientists believed that traveling at a certain speed "could actually make our bones fall apart." So far, that hasn't happened. While adjusting to the future is often alarming, as Bilton illustrated, humans find a way to cope.

A recent article in the Pittsburgh Post-Gazette drove that point home.

Doctors have identified the condition of "text neck," found most often in teenagers and young adults who stare down at their smartphones for two to four hours a day. An orthopedic surgeon quoted in the article advises people to simply "take a break from that thing." If that proves unrealistic, there's a Pilates class geared for teenagers, which includes a focus on overcoming "text neck." The instructor noticed that four girls in a recent class "could not drop their heads in a relaxed position during the exercises" a clear sign of TN.

It is beyond doubt that the proliferation of digital devices is changing the way people process information: smaller gulps from wider sources, less sustained attention. When you can pry your hands from your own smartphone for a minute, go ahead and wring them over this decline in intellectual capacity.

But the endurance of the human species is testimony to its remarkable ability to adapt. And there's one constant: Each generation is horrified by the decadence of the one following.

The Pittsburgh Post-Gazette

More:
Another Viewpoint: Watch your neck: Physiology and the advent of the smartphone - News Chief