Some COVID metrics improve in Colorado, but experts concerned about high rate of hospital admissions – coloradopolitics.com

The average number of COVID-19 cases identified daily in Colorado has fallen by nearly a third since late April, a sign that the state is heading "in a positive direction," one expert said. But another warned that continued high levels of hospital admissions means that the fourth pandemic wave is not yet over.

On April 25, Colorado was averaging 1,731 new COVID-19 cases each day, the most since Jan. 19. But the numbers have fallen nearly every day since then, and the state has averaged 1,182 new cases over the past week. What's more, the average positivity rate, which had topped 6% for the first time since Jan. 14, has also dropped consistently and is now back below 5%.

"Colorado is heading in a positive direction again, and vaccination coverage is almost certainly playing a key role in this decline," said Glen Mays, chair of the Colorado School of Public Health's department of health systems.

"The weather and human behavior are also trending toward safer situations in terms of well-ventilated settings and interactions."

While the numbers are a heartening sign, there are other, more worrying metrics. Elizabeth Carlton, also of the school of public health, said she most closely tracked hospitalizations and noted that Colorado was averaging roughly 100 new COVID-19 hospital admissions each day since April 28.

As of Monday afternoon, there were 653 Coloradans hospitalized with COVID, plus another 50 suspected to have the disease. Though that's lower than two days previously, it remains higher than nearly any other point since late January.

"What does that mean?" Carlton said in an email. "That the number of people developing severe COVID-19 in Colorado doesnt appear to be declining right now."

Dr. Jon Samet, the dean of the public health school, echoed her concern about hospitalizations.

"Case numbers are dropping, but hospitalizations are stable at a plateau in the mid-600s," he wrote in an email to The Gazette on Monday. "Hopefully, since case numbers are a leading indicator, we will see a drop in hospitalizations over the next few weeks."

Carlton and Mays said that vaccinations do appear to be helping prevent infections, as well as hospitalizations and deaths among the state's oldest and most vulnerable residents. Carlton said that without the vaccine, the current situation "would be far worse."

Carlton and other public health experts released a modeling report last week, which looked at data in late April. In it, researchers echoed the clear benefits the vaccine has had on Coloradans most at risk for severe disease and death.

"The benefits of vaccination are clear for older Coloradans," they wrote. "We estimate that approximately 41% of Coloradans overall and >80% of those over age 65 are currently immune due to vaccination and/or prior infection."

But the report also estimated that roughly 1 in 86 Coloradans were infectious then, "much higher than mid-March." The virus was more prevalent in Colorado than it had been since December, as of the report's data analysis in late April.

Researchers wrote that infection control meaning measures taken to blunt the spread, like masking and social distancing had fallen to 56%, which is also lower than it had been in weeks. Mobility, meaning the amount of time people spent away from home, "is reaching its highest levels since the start of the pandemic."

Given the low infection control, the researchers warned that, should that trend continue, "the epidemic curve will not decline to previous lower levels until after August."

Carlton said that though she hoped for a decline in severe COVID cases, "there are ways we could see further increase in severe COVID-19 and infections." She said the state is at a "critical point in terms of vaccinations," particularly with the news Monday that federal regulators cleared the way for Americans between the ages of 12 and 15 to be vaccinated. Carlton called that "a game changer."

"The bad news is that vaccine demand in adults has slowed," she added. "If Colorado can achieve high levels of vaccination across all eligible age groups, then we should see hospitalizations and infections decline in the weeks ahead. If those that are currently unvaccinated remain unvaccinated, a return to pre-pandemic policies and behaviors presents the risk of further increase in infections, hospitalizations and deaths."

More here:
Some COVID metrics improve in Colorado, but experts concerned about high rate of hospital admissions - coloradopolitics.com

Even 85,000 Years Later, We Can Still See How Early Humans Shaped The Land With Fire – ScienceAlert

Fields of rust-colored soil, spindly cassava, small farms, and villages dot the landscape. Dust and smoke blur the mountains visible beyond massive Lake Malawi. Here in tropical Africa, you can't escape the signs of human presence.

How far back in time would you need to go in this place to discover an entirely natural environment?

Our work has shown that it would be a very long time indeed at least 85,000 years, eight times earlier than the world's first land transformations via agriculture.

We are part of an interdisciplinary collaboration between archaeologists who study past human behavior, geochronologists who study the timing of landscape change, and paleoenvironmental scientists who study ancient environments.

By combining evidence from these research specialities, we have identified an instance in the very distant past of early humans bending environments to suit their needs. In doing so, they transformed the landscape around them in ways still visible today.

The dry season is the best time to do archaeological fieldwork here, and finding sites is easy.

Most places we dig in these red soils, we find stone artifacts. They are evidence that someone sat and skillfully broke stones to create edges so sharp they can still draw blood.

Many of these stone tools can be fit back together, reconstructing a single action by a single person, from tens of thousands of years ago.

So far we've recovered more than 45,000 stone artifacts here, buried many feet (1 to 7 meters) below the surface of the ground.

The sites we are excavating date to a time ranging from about 315,000 to 30,000 years ago known as the Middle Stone Age. This was also a period in Africa when innovations in human behavior and creativity pop up frequently and earlier than anywhere else in the world.

How did these artifacts get buried? Why are there so many of them? And what were these ancient hunter-gatherers doing as they made them? To answer these questions, we needed to figure out more about what was happening in this place during their time.

For a clearer picture of the environments where these early humans lived, we turned to the fossil record preserved in layers of mud at the bottom of Lake Malawi.

Over millennia, pollen blown into the water and tiny lake-dwelling organisms became trapped in layers of muck on the lake's floor.

Members of our collaborative team extracted a 1,250-foot (380-meter) drill core of mud from a modified barge, then painstakingly tallied the microscopic fossils it contained, layer by layer.They then used them to reconstruct ancient environments across the entire basin.

Today, this region is characterized by bushy, fire-tolerant open woodlands that do not develop a thick and enclosed canopy.

Forests that do develop these canopies harbor the richest diversity in vegetation; this ecosystem is now restricted to patches that occur at higher elevations. But these forests once stretched all the way to the lakeshore.

Based on the fossil plant evidence present at various times in the drill cores, we could see that the area around Lake Malawi repeatedly alternated between wet times of forest expansion and dry periods of forest contraction.

As the area underwent cycles of aridity, driven by natural climate change, the lake shrank at times to only 5 percent of its present volume. When lake levels eventually rose each time, forests encroached on the shoreline. This happened time and time again over the last 636,000 years.

The mud in the core also contains a record of fire history, in the form of tiny fragments of charcoal. Those little flecks told us that around 85,000 years ago, something strange happened around Lake Malawi. Charcoal production spiked, erosion increased, and, for the first time in more than half a million years, rainfall did not bring forest recovery.

At the same time, this charcoal burst appears in the drill core record, our sites began to show up in the archaeological record eventually becoming so numerous that they formed one continuous landscape littered with stone tools.

Another drill core immediately offshore showed that as site numbers increased, more and more charcoal was washing into the lake.

Early humans had begun to make their first permanent mark on the landscape.

Fire use is a technology that stretches back at least a million years. Using it in such a transformative way is human innovation at its most powerful. Modern hunter-gatherers use fire to warm themselves, cook food and socialize, but many also deploy it as an engineering tool.

Based on the wide-scale and permanent transformation of vegetation into more fire-tolerant woodlands, we infer that this was what these ancient hunter-gatherers were doing.

By converting the natural seasonal rhythm of wildfire into something more controlled, people can encourage specific areas of vegetation to grow at different stages.

This so-called "pyrodiversity" establishes miniature habitat patches and diversifies opportunities for foraging, kind of like increasing product selection at a supermarket.

Just like today, changing any part of an ecosystem has consequences everywhere else.

With the loss of closed forests in ancient Malawi, the vegetation became dominated by more open woodlands that are resilient to fire but these did not contain the same species diversity.

This combination of rainfall and reduced tree cover also increased opportunities for erosion, which spread sediments into a thick blanket known as an alluvial fan. It sealed away archaeological sites and created the landscape you can see here today.

Although the spread of farmers through Africa within the last few thousand years brought about more landscape and vegetation transformations, we have found that the legacy of human impacts was already in place tens of thousands of years before. This offers a chance to understand how such impacts can be sustained over very long timescales.

Most people associate human impacts with a time after the Industrial Revolution, but paleo-scientists have a deeper perspective.

With it, researchers like us can see that wherever and whenever humans lived, we must abandon the idea of "pristine nature," untouched by any human imprint. However, we can also see how humans shaped their environments in sustainable ways over very long periods, causing ecosystem transformation without collapse.

Seeing the long arc of human influence, therefore, gives us much to consider about not only our past, but also our future.

By establishing long-term ecological patterns, conservation efforts related to fire control, species protection and human food security can be more targeted and effective.

People living in the tropics, such as Malawi today, are especially vulnerable to the economic and social impacts of food insecurity brought about by climate change.

By studying the deep past, we can establish connections between long-term human presence and the biodiversity that sustains it.

With this knowledge, people can be better equipped to do what humans had already innovated nearly 100,000 years ago in Africa: manage the world around us.

Jessica Thompson, Assistant Professor of Anthropology, Yale University; David K. Wright, Professor of Archaeology, Conservation and History, University of Oslo, and Sarah Ivory, Assistant Professor of Geosciences, Penn State.

This article is republished from The Conversation under a Creative Commons license. Read the original article.

Read the original:
Even 85,000 Years Later, We Can Still See How Early Humans Shaped The Land With Fire - ScienceAlert

Researchers Say They’ve Uncovered a Massive Facebook Bot Farm From the 2020 Election Mother Jones – Mother Jones

Let our journalists help you make sense of the noise: Subscribe to the Mother Jones Daily newsletter and get a recap of news that matters.

A group of security researchers say theyve unmasked a massive bot farm that aimed to shape public opinion onFacebook during the heat of the 2020 presidential election.

According to Paul Bischoff of Comparitech, a British cybersecurity company, the network includes 13,775 unique Facebook accounts that each posted roughly 15 times per month, for an output of more than 50,000 posts a week. Theaccounts appear to have been used for political manipulation, Bischoff says, with roughly half the posts being related to political topics and another 17 percent related to COVID-19. Each account has a profile photo and friends listlikely consisting of other bots, the researchers suggestand theyve joined specific Facebook groups where their posts are more likely to be seen and discussed by legitimate users.

The most-used keyword in the posts was Trump, the researchers found, followed by Biden. The accounts date back at least as far as October 2020, and, in addition to posts discussing specific events in the 2020 US presidential elections, were also active aroundthe California wildfires, protests in Belarus, and US border issues. The researchers were able to determine that the fake accounts were created and controlled using Selenium, software designed to automate web application testing, but that can also be used to mimic human behavior in ways that could be difficult for automated bot detection software to spot.

According to a Comparitech spokesperson,Facebook did not respond toBob Diachenko, an independent cybersecurity expert who helped lead the research, when he attempted to bring the teamss findings to the platforms attention.A Facebook representative said the company would look into a sample of the accounts identified by Comparitech, but declined further comment.

Facebook has become much more active and aggressive at publicly identifying and taking down what it calls coordinated inauthentic behavior operating on the platform since the 2016 Russian election interference operation. Such inauthentic activity, which the company defines as when posters seek to mislead people about who they are and what they are doing while relying on fake accounts, can include government-backed or private efforts. Just last month, the company claims it had removed 1,565 suspect Facebook accounts, along with 141 Instagram accounts, 724 pages, and 63 groups.

The Comparitech researchers were able to see the email addresses that purportedly registered the phony Facebook accounts. While many used mail[.]ru accounts seemingly originating in Russia, the researchers did not allege who was behind the bot farm, or who controlled the unsecured server.

See original here:
Researchers Say They've Uncovered a Massive Facebook Bot Farm From the 2020 Election Mother Jones - Mother Jones

Readers Write: Gun-carry permits imperil public health – Opinions – The Island Now

It is horrifying to think that there are those like Jeffrey Wiesenfeld who are bemoaning the lack of issuing gun carry permits.

Whether it be open-carry or concealed carry permits, they still involve lethal weapons. Gun possession can lead to increased crime, a homicide or accidental death.

A study by The American Journal of Public Health concluded that an individual carrying a gun for self-defense was more likely to be shot during an assault compared to those who did not have a firearm. Criminals carry guns more often if they think someone else is armed.

I do not negate police brutality, particularly as it applies to the black community. However, we cannot group the police into one category. Like anything else, there are good and bad.

Gun possession is a moral issue besides a political, legislative and health issue. The notion that guns are necessary is morally wrong. We are human beings and the messages of fear, a dog-eat-dog practice should not be indicative of human behavior.

Why not change those messages of firearm defense into ones of emphasizing non-violent

Lois A. Schaffer

Great Neck

Originally posted here:
Readers Write: Gun-carry permits imperil public health - Opinions - The Island Now

Fossil apes and human evolution – Science Magazine

A distinctive ancestor

There has been much focus on the evolution of primates and especially where and how humans diverged in this process. It has often been suggested that the last common ancestor between humans and other apes, especially our closest relative, the chimpanzee, was ape- or chimp-like. Almcija et al. review this area and conclude that the morphology of fossil apes was varied and that it is likely that the last shared ape ancestor had its own set of traits, different from those of modern humans and modern apes, both of which have been undergoing separate suites of selection pressures.

Science, this issue p. eabb4363

Ever since the writings of Darwin and Huxley, humans place in nature relative to apes (nonhuman hominoids) and the geographic origins of the human lineage (hominins) have been heavily debated. Humans diverged from apes [specifically, the chimpanzee lineage (Pan)] at some point between ~9.3 million and ~6.5 million years ago (Ma), and habitual bipedalism evolved early in hominins (accompanied by enhanced manipulation and, later on, cognition). To understand the selective pressures surrounding hominin origins, it is necessary to reconstruct the morphology, behavior, and environment of the Pan-Homo last common ancestor (LCA). Top-down approaches have relied on living apes (especially chimpanzees) to reconstruct hominin origins. However, bottom-up perspectives from the fossil record suggest that modern hominoids represent a decimated and biased sample of a larger ancient radiation and present alternative possibilities for the morphology and geography of the Pan-Homo LCA. Reconciling these two views remains at the core of the human origins problem.

There is no consensus on the phylogenetic positions of the diverse and widely distributed Miocene apes. Besides their fragmentary record, disagreements are due to the complexity of interpreting fossil morphologies that present mosaics of primitive and derived features, likely because of parallel evolution (i.e., homoplasy). This has led some authors to exclude known Miocene apes from the modern hominoid radiation. However, most researchers identify some fossil apes as either stem or crown members of the hominid clade [i.e., preceding the divergence between orangutans (pongines) and African great apes and humans (hominines), or as a part of the modern great ape radiation]. European Miocene apes have prominently figured in discussions about the geographic origin of hominines. Kenyapith apes dispersed from Africa into Eurasia ~16 to 14 Ma, and some of them likely gave rise to the European dryopith apes and the Asian pongines before 12.5 Ma. Some authors interpret dryopiths as stem hominines and support their back-to-Africa dispersal in the latest Miocene, subsequently evolving into modern African apes and hominins. Others interpret dryopiths as broadly ancestral to hominids or an evolutionary dead end.

Increased habitat fragmentation during the late Miocene in Africa might explain the evolution of African ape knuckle walking and hominin bipedalism from an orthograde arboreal ancestor. Bipedalism might have allowed humans to escape the great ape specialization trapan adaptive feedback loop between diet, specialized arboreal locomotion, cognition, and life history. However, understanding the different selection pressures that underlie knuckle walking and bipedalism is hindered by locomotor uncertainties about the Pan-Homo LCA and its Miocene forebears. In turn, the functional interpretation of Miocene ape mosaic morphologies is challenging because it depends on the relevance of primitive features. Furthermore, adaptive complexes can be co-opted to perform new functions during evolution. For instance, features that are functionally related to quadrupedalism or orthogrady can be misinterpreted as bipedal adaptations. Miocene apes show that the orthograde body plan, which predates below-branch suspension, is likely an adaptation for vertical climbing that was subsequently co-opted for other orthograde behaviors, including habitual bipedalism.

Future research efforts on hominin origins should focus on (i) fieldwork in unexplored areas where Miocene apes have yet to be found, (ii) methodological advances in morphology-based phylogenetics and paleoproteomics to retrieve molecular data beyond ancient DNA limits, and (iii) modeling driven by experimental data that integrates morphological and biomechanical information, to test locomotor inferences for extinct taxa. It is also imperative to stop assigning a starring role to each new fossil discovery to fit evolutionary scenarios that are not based on testable hypotheses.

Early hominins likely originated in Africa from a Miocene LCA that does not match any living ape (e.g., it might not have been adapted specifically for suspension or knuckle walking). Despite phylogenetic uncertainties, fossil apes remain essential to reconstruct the starting point from which humans and chimpanzees evolved.

Whereas the phylogenetic relationships among living species can be retrieved using genetic data, the position of most extinct species remains contentious. Surprisingly, complete-enough fossils that can be attributed to the gorilla and chimpanzee lineages remain to be discovered. Assuming different positions of available fossil apes (or ignoring them owing to uncertainty) markedly affects reconstructions of key ancestral nodes, such as that of the chimpanzee-human LCA.

Humans diverged from apes (chimpanzees, specifically) toward the end of the Miocene ~9.3 million to 6.5 million years ago. Understanding the origins of the human lineage (hominins) requires reconstructing the morphology, behavior, and environment of the chimpanzee-human last common ancestor. Modern hominoids (that is, humans and apes) share multiple features (for example, an orthograde body plan facilitating upright positional behaviors). However, the fossil record indicates that living hominoids constitute narrow representatives of an ancient radiation of more widely distributed, diverse species, none of which exhibit the entire suite of locomotor adaptations present in the extant relatives. Hence, some modern ape similarities might have evolved in parallel in response to similar selection pressures. Current evidence suggests that hominins originated in Africa from Miocene ape ancestors unlike any living species.

In 1871, Darwin (1) speculated that humans originated in Africa based on the anatomical similarities with African apes (gorillas and chimpanzees) identified by Huxley (2). However, Darwin urged caution until more fossils became availablethe European Dryopithecus was the only recognized fossil ape at the time (3). After 150 years of continuous discoveries, essential information about human origins remains elusive owing to debates surrounding the interpretation of fossil apes (Figs. 1 and 2).

Extant apes live in (or nearby) densely forested areas around the equator in Africa and Southeast Asia. Except for the recently recognized tapanuli orangutan (which may represent a subspecies of the Sumatran orangutan), each of the three extant great ape genera presently has two geographically separated species. The Congo River (highlighted in dark blue) acts as the current barrier between common chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and bonobos (Pan paniscus). Red stars indicate regions with Miocene sediments (spanning ~23 to 5.3 Ma) where fossil apes have been uncovered. (Some regions may contain more than one site; contiguous regions are indicated with different stars if they extend over more than one political zone.) It is possible that modern great ape habitats do not represent the ancestral environments where the great ape and human clade evolved. Paleontologically, the vast majority of Africa, west of the Rift Valley, remains highly unexplored. Extant ape ranges were taken from the International Union for Conservation of Nature (IUCN Red List). Background image sources: Esri, DigitalGlobe, GeoEye, i-cubed, USDA FSA, USGS, AEX, Getmapping, Aerogrid, IGN, IGP, swisstopo, and the GIS user community.

(A) Macaque (above) and chimpanzee (below) in typical postures, showing general differences between pronograde and orthograde body plan characteristics. In comparison to a pronograde monkey, the modern hominoid orthograde body plan is characterized by the lack of an external tail (the coccyx being its vestigial remnant), a ribcage that is mediolaterally broad and dorsoventrally shallow, dorsally placed scapulae that are cranially elevated and oriented, a shorter lower back, and long iliac blades. Modern hominoids have higher ranges of joint mobility, such as the full elbow extension shown here, facilitated by a short ulnar olecranon process. The inset further shows differences in lumbar vertebral anatomy, including more dorsally situated and oriented transverse processes in orthograde hominoids. (B) Representatives of each extant hominoid lineage (left column) show different postural variations associated with an orthograde body plan. The orthograde body plan facilitates bipedal walking in modern humans and different combinations of arboreal climbing and below-branch suspension in apes. Knuckle walking in highly terrestrial African apes is seen as a compromise positional behavior superimposed onto an orthograde ape with long forelimbs relative to the hindlimbs. Associated skeletons of fossil hominoids (right column) show that an orthograde body can be disassociated from specific adaptions for suspension (e.g., Pierolapithecus exhibits shorter and less curved digits than Hispanopithecus). Other fossil apes exhibit primitive monkey-like pronograde body plans with somewhat more modern ape-like forelimbs (e.g., Nacholapithecus). Approximate age in millions of years ago is given to representative fossils of each extinct genus: Ardipithecus (ARA-VP-6/500), Nacholapithecus (KNM-BG35250), Pierolapithecus (IPS21350), Hispanopithecus (IPS18800), and Oreopithecus (IGF 11778). Silhouettes of extant and fossil skeletons are shown at about the same scale.

Genomic data indicate that humans and chimpanzees are sister lineages (hominins and panins, respectively; Box 1) that diverged from a last common ancestor (LCA) toward the end of the Miocene, at some point between ~9.3 million and ~6.5 million years ago (Ma) (4, 5). All extant hominoids (apes and humans) are characterized by the lack of an external tail, high joint mobility (e.g., elbow, wrist, hip), and the possession of an orthograde (upright) body plan, as opposed to the more primitive, pronograde body plan of other anthropoids and most other mammals (Fig. 2). These body plans are associated with two different types of positional (postural and locomotor) behaviors: pronograde behaviors, taking place on nearly horizontal supports with the trunk held roughly horizontally; and orthograde (or antipronograde) behaviors, with the torso positioned vertically (6, 7). Extant ape features also include enhanced joint mobility, long forelimbs relative to hindlimbs, and (except gorillas) long hands with high-to-very-high finger curvature (810). The orthograde body plan is generally interpreted as a suspensory adaptation (11, 12), or as an adaptation for vertical climbing subsequently co-opted for suspension (13).

The adjectives lesser and great refer to the smaller size of the former relative to great apes and human group, not to old evolutionary notions based on the Scala Naturae. Given that some apes are more closely related to humans than to other apes, the word ape is a gradistic term used here informally to refer to all nonhominin hominoids. Finally, the taxonomic convention used (the most common), does not reflect that panins and hominins are monophyletic [although some do; e.g., (169)].

Order Primates

Suborder Strepsirrhini (non-tarsier prosimians: lemurs, galagos and lorises)

Suborder Haplorrhini (tarsiers and simians)

Infraorder Tarsiiformes (tarsiers)

Infraorder Simiiformes (or Anthropoidea: simians or anthropoids)

Parvorder Platyrrhini (New World monkeys)

Parvorder Catarrhini (Old World simians)

Superfamily Cercopithecoidea (Old World monkeys)

Superfamily Hominoidea (apes and humans)

Family Hylobatidae (lesser apes: gibbons and siamangs)

Family Hominidae (great apes and humans)

Subfamily Ponginae (the orangutan lineage)

Genus Pongo (orangutans)

Subfamily Homininae (the African ape and human lineage)

Tribe Gorillini (the gorilla lineage)

Genus Gorilla (gorillas)

Tribe Panini (the chimpanzee lineage)

Genus Pan (common chimpanzees and bonobos)

Tribe Hominini (the human lineage)

Genus Homo (humans)

Based on similarities between chimpanzees and gorillas, a prevalent evolutionary model argues that African apes represent living fossils and that knuckle-walking chimpanzees closely reflect the morphology and behavior of the Pan-Homo LCAthe starting point of human evolution (14, 15). This working paradigm also postulates that modern African apes occupy the same habitats as their ancestors (16) (Fig. 1). This assumption is based on a classical scenario that situates hominin origins in East Africa, owing to environmental changes after the rifting of East African Rift Valley during the Miocene (17). For some, a chimpanzee-like Pan-Homo LCA could also imply that all extant ape locomotor adaptations were inherited from a modern ape-like ancestor (18). However, the fossil record denotes a more complex picture: Miocene apes often display mosaic morphologies, and even those interpreted as crown hominoids do not exhibit all the features present in living apes (19) (Fig. 3).

A time-calibrated phylogenetic tree of living hominoids is depicted next to the spatiotemporal ranges of the fossil hominoids mentioned in the text. Fossil taxa are color coded based on possible phylogenetic hypotheses. The vertical green dashed line indicates that there is a continuity in the African fossil ape record. However, currently, it is sparse between ~14 and 10 Ma. Robust and lasting phylogenetic inferences of apes are difficult, in part, because of the fragmentary nature of the fossil record and probable high levels of homoplasy. Many Miocene ape taxa are represented only by fragmentary dentognathic fossils, and the utility of mandibles and molars for inferring phylogeny in apes has been questioned. Another area of uncertainty relates to the position of many early and middle Miocene African apes relative to the crown hominoid node. The discovery or recognition of more complete early Miocene fossil hylobatids would help resolve their position and, thus, what really defines the great ape and human family. Splitting times are based on the molecular clock estimates of Springer et al. (168) (hominoids and hominids) and Moorjani et al. (4), which are more updated for hominines and Pan-Homo. Silhouettes are not to scale. Shaded boxes represent geographic distributions (green is Africa, gold is Europe, and purple is Asia).

The Pan-like LCA model builds on the East Side Story of hominin origins (17), a seriously challenged scenario. First, it is grounded in the living-ape geographic distribution, which may not match that at the time of the Pan-Homo split (Fig. 1). Second, the model relies on an outdated account of the fossil record (from the 1980s), when the earliest known hominin (Australopithecus afarensis) was recorded in East Africa, and no possible fossil gorillas and chimpanzees were known (17). Subsequent fossil discoveries are incompatible with such a narrative: Australopithecus remains from Chad indicate that early hominins were living ~2500 km west of the East African Rift ~3.5 Ma (20). Furthermore, if Sahelanthropus is a hominin, it would push back the human lineage presence in north-central Africa to ~7 Ma (21). Moreover, continued fieldwork efforts in less explored areas have shown that hominoids lived across Afro-Arabia during the Miocene (2225). In addition, remains of putative hominines have been found in East Africa (26, 27), perhaps even in Europe (28, 29). Finally, paleoenvironmental reconstructions for late Miocene apes and hominins suggest that the Pan-Homo LCA inhabited woodlands, not tropical rainforests (3033).

Current debates about the transition from an ape into a bipedal hominin are centered on the morphological and locomotor reconstruction of the Pan-Homo LCA, as well as its paleobiogeography. Discrepancies are caused by conflicting evolutionary signals among living and fossil hominoids, indicating rampant homoplasy (independent evolution causing false homology), and are further complicated by the highly incomplete and fragmentary nature of the hominoid fossil record. This review argues that, despite the limitations, the information provided by fossil apes is essential to inform evolutionary scenarios of human origins.

Since Linnaeus established modern taxonomy in 1758 (34) and until the 1960s, morphological similarity was the main basis for classifying organisms. Linnaeus included modern humans (Homo sapiens) within the order Primates, but it was not until 1863 that Huxley provided the first systematic review of differences and similarities between humans and apes (2). Imagining himself as a scientific Saturnian, Huxley stated that, The structural differences between Man and the Man-like apes certainly justify our regarding him as constituting a family apart from them; though, inasmuch as he differs less from them than they do from other families of the same order, there can be no justification for placing him in a distinct order [(2), p. 104]. Huxleys work was motivated by widespread claims (e.g., Cuvier, Owen) that humans uniqueness warranted their placement in a separate order. Darwin concurred with Huxley that humans should be classified in their own family within primates (1).

We now know that most human features are primitive traits inherited from primate (e.g., trichromatic stereoscopic vision, manual grasping) or earlier (e.g., five digits) ancestors (35). Even humans distinctively large brains and delayed maturation are framed within a primate trend of increased encephalization and slower life history compared with other mammals (35, 36). Some differences in brain size may partly reflect a neocortex enlargement related to enhanced visual and grasping abilities (37). Like extant great apes, humans display larger body size, larger relative brain size, a slower life-history profile, and more elaborate cognitive abilities than other primates (hylobatids included) (36). However, modern humans are extreme outliers in terms of delayed maturation, encephalization, advanced cognition, and manual dexterity, ultimately leading to symbolic language and technology (38).

Anatomically, only two adaptive complexes represent synapomorphies present in all hominins: the loss of the canine honing complex and features related to habitual bipedalism (33, 39). Most anthropoids possess large and sexually dimorphic canines coupled with body size differences between males and females, reflecting levels of agonistic behavior and sociosexual structure (40). The fossil record indicates that there was a reduction in canine height, leading to the loss of the honing complex in early hominins (41). Habitual bipedalism is reflected in several traits across the body (e.g., foramen magnum position and orientation; pelvic, lower-back, and lower-limb morphology), present (or inferred) in the earliest hominins (21, 33, 42).

Darwin linked the origin of bipedalism with an adaptive complex related to freeing the hands from locomotion to use and make tools (replacing large canines), leading to a reciprocal feedback loop involving brain size, cognition, culture, and, eventually, civilization (1). Multiple variants in the order of these events have been advocated, with the freeing of the hands alternatively linked to tools (43), food acquisition and carrying (15), or provisioning within a monogamous social structure (44), to name a few. There is general agreement that canine reduction (including social structure changes), enhanced manipulative capabilities, and bipedalism were interrelated during human evolution. However, determining the order of events and their causality requires reconstructing the ape-human LCA from which hominins originated. Darwin also speculated that humans and modern African ape ancestors originated in Africa (1), based on the anatomical similarities identified by Huxley and his own observations that many living mammals are closely related to extinct species of the same region. However, given the limited ape fossil record at that time, he concluded that it was useless to speculate on this subject [(1), p. 199]. Using the French Dryopithecus to calibrate his clock, Darwin concluded that humans likely diverged as early as the Eocene and warned against the error of supposing that the early progenitor of the whole Simian stock, including man, was identical with, or even closely resembled, any existing ape or monkey [(1), p. 199]. These ideas inaugurated a century of discussions about humans place in nature.

Until the 1950s, the geographic origin of hominins was disputed between Africa, Asia, and Europe. After the publication of Darwins On the Origin of Species (45), Haeckel predicted that the missing link (dubbed Pithecanthropus, the ape-man) would be found in Asia (46). This idea led to Dubois 1891 discovery of Homo erectus in Indonesia (47). In 1925, Dart published the discovery of Australopithecus africanus, the man-ape from South Africa (48). However, the scientific community still focused on Europe because of the Piltdown fossils, until they were exposed as a hoax (49). Asia remained a mother continent contender owing to the man-like ape Ramapithecus, discovered in the Indian Siwaliks (50).

During this time, the relationships of humans to other primates were highly contentious. Most authors advocated an ancient divergence of humans from apes (51, 52) or favored a closer relationship to the great apes than to the lesser apes (53, 54). A few proposed that humans were more closely related to one or both of the African apes (55, 56), although these views were not widely accepted (57). These alternative phylogenetic hypotheses heavily affected reconstructions of the LCA. Some (e.g., Schultz, Straus) advocated for a generalized ape ancestor (52), whereas others relied on extant hominoid models. Notably, Keith developed a scenario in which a hylobatian brachiating stage preceded an African ape-like creature: a knuckle-walking troglodytian phase immediately preceding bipedalism (11). Focused on Keiths hylobatian stage, Morton proposed that the vertically suspended posture of a small-bodied hylobatid-like ancestor caused the erect posture of human bipedalism (12). Gregory, another prominent brachiationist, supported similar views (53). Morton argued that knuckle walking did not represent an intermediate stage preceding bipedalism but rather a reversion toward quadrupedalism in large-bodied apes specialized for brachiation. At that time, brachiation was used for any locomotion in which the body was suspended by the hands. Now, it refers to the pendulum-like arm-swinging locomotion of hylobatids (6).

By the 1960s, the Leakeys discoveries in Tanzania [e.g., Paranthropus boisei (58), Homo habilis (59)] reinforced the relevance of Africa in human evolution, which became firmly established as the mother continent with the A. afarensis discoveries during the 1970s (60, 61). LCA models still centered on the available fossil apes (mostly represented by jaw fragments and isolated teeth) accumulated after decades of paleontological fieldwork in Africa and Eurasia. In 1965, Simons and Pilbeam (62) revised and organized available Miocene apes in three genera: Dryopithecus, Gigantopithecus, and Ramapithecus. The genus Sivapithecus was included in Dryopithecus, considered the ancestor of African apes, whereas Ramapithecus was considered ancestral to humans based on its short face (and inferred small canines) (63). Leakey (64) and others agreed with Simons and Pilbeam that humans belong to their own family (Hominidae, or hominids), whereas great apes would belong to a distinct family (Pongidae, or pongids). He also agreed that Ramapithecus was an Asian early human ancestor. However, Leakey proposed reserving the genus Sivapithecus for the Asian dryopithecines and claimed that the human lineage could be traced back to, at least, the middle Miocene of Africa with Kenyapithecus wickeri (~14 Ma).

Two major revolutions in the study of evolutionary relationships started in the 1960s. First, a series of studies jump-started the field of molecular anthropology: Blood protein comparisons by Zuckerkandl et al. (65) and Goodman (66) found that some great apesgorillas and chimpanzeeswere more closely related to humans than to orangutans. Sarich and Wilson developed an immunological molecular clock and concluded that African apes and humans share a common ancestor as recent as ~5 Ma (67). These results led to decades-long debates regarding the African apehuman split. For example, Washburn resurrected extant African apes as ancestral models in human evolution, proposing knuckle walking as the precursor of terrestrial bipedalism (68). By contrast, paleontologists argued that the molecular clock was inaccurate because of the much older age of the purported human ancestors Kenyapithecus and Ramapithecus (69). Second, Hennigian cladistics (phylogenetic systematics), which only recognizes synapomorphies (shared derived features) as informative for reconstructing phylogeny (70), became slowly implemented in anthropology by the mid-1970s (71).

In the 1970s and 1980s, the relationships among gorillas, chimpanzees, and humans were still disputed. Chromosomal comparisons (72), DNA hybridization (73), and hemoglobin sequencing (74) supported a closer relationship between chimpanzees and humans, whereas morphology-based cladistics recovered gorilla-chimpanzee as monophyletic (75). In the late 1980s, the first single-locus DNA sequencing studies (76), followed in the 1990s with multiple loci analyses, finally resolved the trichotomy (77). Current genomic evidence indicates that humans are more closely related to chimpanzees (5), having diverged at some time between ~9.3 and ~6.5 Ma (4). Ever since the molecular revolution, the perceived relevance of fossil apes in human evolution has been in jeopardy.

Extant African apes have been considered ancestral models since Keiths troglodytian stage in the 1920s (11), and especially since the 1960s, with updated hypotheses inspired by the molecular revolution (68, 78) and field discoveries on chimpanzee behavior by Goodall (79). Leakey played a central role in promoting Goodalls pioneering research (subsequently fostering Fosseys research in gorillas and Galdikass research in orangutans). Now, a prominent paradigm proposes that chimpanzees represent living fossils that closely depict the Pan-Homo LCA (14, 16). This model combines molecular data with the anachronistic view that Gorilla and Pan are morphologically similar (75). Under these assumptions, knuckle walking, once used to defend African ape monophyly (80), is used to argue that African apes are morphologically conservative and only display size-related differences (14). This model contends that gorillas are allometrically enlarged chimps and that chimpanzees [or bonobos (78)] constitute a suitable model for the Pan-Homo LCA, perhaps even the hominine or hominid LCAs (14). This narrative also incorporates the paleobiogeographic assumption that African apes likely occupy the same habitats as their ancestors: Without new selection pressures, there was no need for evolution.

If hominins originated from a chimpanzee-like LCA, human bipedalism must have evolved from knuckle walking (15), a functional compromise enabling terrestrial travel while retaining climbing adaptations (80). Under this view, bipedal hominins originated from an ancestor that was already terrestrial while traveling. These conclusions are logical from a top-down perspective, based on the evidence provided by extant hominoids and early hominins. However, a fully informed theory of hominin origins must also apply a bottom-up approach (81, 82), from the perspective of extinct apes preceding the Pan-Homo split. It is also essential to clarify whether chimpanzees represent a good ancestral model for the Pan-Homo LCA. Unfortunately, the view from the bottom is blurry.

With more than 50 hominoid genera and a broad geographic distribution (Fig. 1), the Miocene has been dubbed the real planet of the apes (83). Besides their fragmentary nature, a persistent challenge is understanding the phylogenetic relationships among fossil apes, which exhibit mosaics of primitive and derived features with no modern analogs. The Asian Miocene ape Sivapithecus best exemplifies this complexity. Discoveries during the 1970s and 1980s, including a facial skeleton (84), clarified that Ramapithecus is a junior synonym of Sivapithecus, which is likely related to orangutans (85). However, two Sivapithecus humeri show a primitive (pronograde-related) morphology, calling into question the close phylogenetic link with Pongo that had been inferred from facial similarities (86).

The root of this Sivapithecus dilemma (18) is identifying where phylogenetic signal is best captured in hominoids: the postcranium or the cranium? The former implies that a Pongo-like face evolved independently twice; the latter entails that some postcranial similarities among living apes evolved more than once. Both hypotheses highlight the phylogenetic noise that homoplasy introduces in phylogenetic inference. Indeed, several studies have found that homoplasy similarly affects both anatomical areas (87). The conclusion that Sivapithecus is not a pongine relies on the assumption that suspensory adaptations and other orthograde-related features present in living hominoids were inherited from their LCA (18). However, this is contradicted by differences among living apes [e.g., forelimb and hand anatomy, degree of limb elongation, hip abduction capability (8, 9, 19, 80, 8891)]. These studies concluded that apparent similarities could represent independently evolved biomechanical solutions to similar locomotor selection pressures. For instance, hand length similarities among living apes result from different combinations of metacarpal and/or phalangeal elongation in each extant genus (9).

Parallel evolutionhomoplasy among closely related taxa due to shared genetic and developmental pathwayscould explain some postcranial similarities related to suspensory behaviors among extant apes (80). Compared with convergences among distantly related taxa, parallelisms are more subtle and difficult to detect and they readily evolve when similar selection pressures appear. Within extant primates, suspensory adaptions evolved independently in atelines and between hylobatids and great apes (8, 80, 88, 91, 92). When the hominoid fossil record is added, independent evolution of suspensory adaptations has been inferred, too, for orangutans, chimpanzees, and some extinct lineages (9, 89, 93, 94). Knuckle walking has also been proposed to have different origins in gorillas and chimpanzees (80, 93, 95). As for suspension, the preexistence of an orthograde body plan, vertical climbing, and general arboreal heritage could have facilitated the independent evolution of knuckle walking to circumvent similar biomechanical demands during terrestrial quadrupedalism while preserving a powerful grasping hand suitable for arboreal locomotion (9).

The possibility of parallelisms indicates that ancestral nodes in the hominoid evolutionary tree, including the Pan-Homo LCA, cannot be readily inferred without incorporating fossils. In addition, fossils from known evolutionary lineages are commonly used to calibrate molecular clocks despite being subject to considerable uncertainty (4). Even worse, relatively complete fossil apes undisputedly assigned to early members of the gorilla and chimpanzee lineages remain to be found.

Sivapithecus and other fossil Asian great apes (e.g., Khoratpithecus, Ankarapithecus, Lufengpithecus) are generally considered pongines (Fig. 3) based on derived craniodental traits shared with Pongo (94, 9698), although alternative views exist, particularly for Lufengpithecus (99). By contrast, the phylogenetic positions of apes from the African early (e.g., Ekembo, Morotopithecus) and middle Miocene (Kenyapithecus, Nacholapithecus, Equatorius) remain very controversial. Like Sivapithecus, they exhibit only some modern hominoid features superimposed onto a primitive-looking pronograde (monkey-like) body plan (Fig. 2). Some authors interpret this mosaicism as indicating that most Miocene apes do not belong within the crown hominoid radiation and, thus, are irrelevant to reconstructions of the Pan-Homo LCA (14). This is likely the case for early Miocene African taxa. However, the vertebrae of Morotopithecus [~20 Ma (100) or ~17 Ma (101)] display orthogrady-related features absent from other stem hominoids, indicating either a closer relationship with crown hominoids or an independent evolution of orthogrady (102). In turn, Kenyapithecus and Nacholapithecus are commonly regarded as preceding the pongine-hominine split owing to the possession of some modern hominid craniodental synapomorphies combined with a more primitive postcranium than that of living great apes (94, 103). This raises the question: Can some Miocene apes belong to the crown hominid clade despite lacking many of the features shared by extant great apes?

The large-bodied apes from the middle-to-late Miocene of Europe are at the center of discussions about great ape and human evolution (19, 28, 94, 104, 105). Named after Dryopithecus (3), they are generally distinguished as a subfamily (Dryopithecinae) (94) or tribe (Dryopithecini) (28). However, it is unclear if they constitute a monophyletic group or a paraphyletic assemblage of stem and crown hominoids (94). Thus, we refer to them informally as dryopiths. These apes are dentally conservative, but each genus exhibits different cranial and postcranial morphology. The dryopith fossil record includes the oldest skeletons that consistently exhibit postcranial features of living hominoids (orthograde body plan and/or long and more curved digits). Dryopithecus (~12 to 11 Ma) is known from craniodental remains and isolated postcranials that are too scarce to reconstruct its overall anatomy (106). By contrast, Pierolapithecus (~12 Ma) is represented by a cranium with an associated partial skeleton (19). Cranially a great ape, its rib, clavicle, lumbar, and wrist morphologies are unambiguous evidence of an orthograde body plan. Yet, unlike chimpanzees and orangutans (but similar to gorillas), Pierolapithecus lacks specialized below-branch suspensory adaptations [see discussion in (10)]. The recently described Danuvius (~11.6 Ma, Germany), and the slightly younger (~10 to 9 Ma) Hispanopithecus (Spain) (105) and Rudapithecus (Hungary) (28) represent the oldest record of specialized below-branch suspensory adaptations (e.g., long and strongly curved phalanges; Fig. 2). Danuvius has also been argued to show adaptations to habitual bipedalism (but see below).

The different mosaic morphology exhibited by each dryopith genus is a major challenge for deciphering their phylogenetic relationships (Fig. 3). Current competing phylogenetic hypotheses consider dryopiths as stem hominoids (107, 108), stem hominids (94, 96, 109), or crown hominids closer to either pongines (105), hominines (28), or even hominins (29, 110). However, recent phylogenetic analyses of apes recovered dryopiths as stem hominids (97, 109), perhaps except Ouranopithecus (~9 to 8 Ma) and Graecopithecus (~7 Ma) (97). Ouranopithecus has been interpreted by some as a stem hominine, or even as a crown member more closely related to the gorilla or human lineages (110). Graecopithecus has also been advocated as a hominin (29), although the fragmentary available material hinders evaluation of this hypothesis. Such contrasting views about dryopiths stem from their incomplete and fragmentary fossil record coupled with pervasive homoplasy. However, because these factors are equal for all researchers, their different conclusions must also relate to analytical differences (e.g., taxonomy, sampling, polymorphic and continuous trait treatment). The root of the conflict is the remarkable differences in subjective definition and scoring of complex morphologies (e.g., incipient supraorbital torus).

One hundred fifty years after Darwin speculated that modern African ape and human ancestors originated in Africa, possible hominins have been found as far back as the latest Miocene of Africa (21, 33, 111): Sahelanthropus (~7 Ma), Orrorin (~6 Ma), and Ardipithecus kadabba (~5.8 to 5.2 Ma). However, others question the feasibility of identifying the earliest hominins among the diverse Miocene apes (96, 112). Puzzlingly, despite some claims based on scarce remains (113115), ancient representatives of the gorilla and chimpanzee lineages remain elusive. Some apes from the African late MioceneChororapithecus (26), Nakalipithecus (27), and Samburupithecus (116)have been interpreted as hominines, but the available fragmentary remains preclude a conclusive assessment. Furthermore, Samburupithecus is likely a late-occurring stem hominoid (97, 117).

During the middle Miocene (~16.5 to 14 Ma), apes are first found out of Africa. These are the genera Kenyapithecus (Turkey) and Griphopithecus (Turkey and central Europe). We informally refer to them as the kenyapiths because there is no consensus on their relationships (28, 94, 118). Kenyapiths indicate that putative stem hominids are first recorded in Eurasia and Africa before the earliest record of both European dryopiths and Asian pongines at ~12.5 Ma (94). Paleobiogeographical and paleontological data suggest that kenyapiths dispersed from Africa into Eurasia as one of the multiple catarrhine intercontinental dispersal events occurred during the Miocene (e.g., hylobatids, pliopithecoids) (83, 94). Although some competing evolutionary scenarios agree that kenyapiths gave rise to dryopiths in Europe, the phylogenetic and geographic origin of hominines remains contentious (28, 94).

If dryopiths are stem hominids, they could either be close to the crown group or constitute an evolutionary dead end, an independent experiment not directly related to either pongines or hominines. Alternatively, dryopiths might be crown hominids more closely related to one of these groups. If dryopiths are hominines, this implies that the latter could have originated in Europe and subsequently dispersed back to Africa during the late Miocene (28, 29, 83). This would coincide with vegetation structure changes caused by a trend of increased cooling and seasonality (32) that ultimately drove European apes to extinction [or back to Africa (28)]. In this scenario, hominines and pongines would be vicariant groups that originally evolved in Europe and Asia, respectively, from early kenyapith ancestors. Given the suspensory specializations of late Miocene dryopiths (Hispanopithecus and Rudapithecus), if modern African apes originated from these forms, this scenario implies that the hominine ancestor could have been more reliant on suspension than living chimpanzees or gorillas. The claim that hominines originated outside of Africa may be justified by cladistic analyses recovering dryopiths as stem hominines but may not be based on the lack of late Miocene great apes in Africa because fossils from this critical time period have been discovered (~13 to 7 Ma) (Fig. 3). Both molecular and paleontological evidence (e.g., Sivapithecus) situate the pongine-hominine divergence within the middle Miocene. Hence, the debate cannot be settled without more conclusively resolving the phylogenetic relationships of middle Miocene dryopiths.

An alternative scenario proposes a vicariant divergence for hominines and pongines from kenyapith ancestors but favors the origin of hominines in Africa (94, 119). It argues for a second vicariant event between European dryopiths and Asian pongines soon after the kenyapith dispersal into Eurasia. Cladistically, dryopiths would be pongines but would share none of the currently recognized pongine autapomorphies, evolved after the second vicariant event. This scenario is difficult to test, but it would be consistent with the apparent absence of clear pongine synapomorphies in Lufengpithecus (99) and the more derived nasoalveolar morphology of Nacholapithecus (103) compared with some dryopiths (106). However, it would imply even higher levels of homoplasy, including the independent acquisition of an orthograde body plan in Africa and Eurasia from pronograde kenyapith ancestors.

A third possibility is that none of the taxa discussed above are closely related to the African ape and human clade (107). Under this view, bona fide extinct nonhominin hominines have yet to be found in largely unexplored regions of Africa, explaining the virtual lack of a gorilla and chimpanzee fossil record. According to Pilbeam, paleoanthropologists could be like the drunk looking for his keys under the lamppost where it was light rather than where he had dropped them, working with what we had rather than asking whether or not that was adequate [(108), pp. 155156]. Africa is a huge continent, and most paleontological discoveries are concentrated in a small portion of it. The greatest challenge is finding hominoid-bearing Mio-Pliocene sites outside East and South Africa, even though we know they exist (2022). Besides insufficient sampling effort, this is hindered by numerous impediments to fieldwork in most of Africa, including geopolitical conflicts, restricted land use development, lack of suitable outcrops (due to extensive vegetation cover), and taphonomic factors [tropical forests do not favor fossil preservation (120)].

The decades-long feud regarding arboreality and bipedalism in A. afarensis exemplifies the complexity of inferring function from anatomy. Totalist functional morphologists rely on a species total morphological pattern (121) to infer its locomotor repertoire. Totalists see a bipedal early hominin with some ape-like retentions (e.g., curved fingers) pointing to continued use of the trees and consider that certain not-yet-human-like features (e.g., hip) indicate a different type of bipedalism (122). Instead, directionalistsfor whom functional inferences are only possible for derived traits evolved for a specific functionfocus exclusively on bipedal adaptations (123). Totalist and directionalist interpretations of the fossil record differ in the adaptive significance attributed to primitive features, which result in different behavioral reconstructions. Two other related factors further complicate locomotor inferences in extinct species: First, different positional behaviors have similar mechanical demands [e.g., bipedalism, quadrupedalism and some types of climbing (39)]. Second, preexisting morphofunctional complexes originally selected to fulfill a particular function (adaptations) can be subsequently co-opted for a new role (exaptations).

The mosaic nature of hominoid morphological evolution makes the functional reconstruction of fossil apes especially challenging, as recently exemplified by Danuvius (104): It was described as possessing long and curved fingers, a long and flexible vertebral column, hip and knee joints indicative of extended postures, and an ankle configuration aligning the foot perpendicular to the long axis of the tibia. Such a combination of features was functionally interpreted as indicating below-branch suspension combined with above-branch bipedalism. However, a critique to the original study concluded that the morphological affinities of Danuvius with modern great apes support a positional repertoire that includes orthogrady and suspension, but not bipedalism (124). Part of the problem with the original interpretation is that it infers a derived locomotor behaviorbipedalismfrom primitive features that are also functionally related to quadrupedalism. For instance, the inferred long-back morphology of Danuvius is characteristic of most quadrupedal monkeys and other Miocene apes (125), denoting the lack of trunk specialization seen in extant great apes. The Danuvius femoral head joint, being (primitively) posterosuperiorly expanded (126), is consistent with flexed quadrupedal hip postures that are not used during human-like bipedalism. In addition, the distal tibia configuration of Danuvius is shared with Ekembo and cercopithecoids (104), thus being likely plesiomorphic and not unique to bipeds. When the primitive and derived features of Danuvius are considered, a totalist would argue that it combined high degrees of plesiomorphic quadrupedal locomotion with novel (suspensory) behaviors, whereas a directionalist would downplay the primitive features in favor of the newly derived adaptive traits (i.e., suspension).

The late Miocene Oreopithecus (~7 Ma, Italy) is another example of conflicting phylogenetic and functional signals. Phylogenetic interpretations of Oreopithecus include cercopithecoid, stem hominoid, and hominid (even hominin) status (127). However, current phylogenetic analyses suggest that Oreopithecus could represent a late-occurring stem hominoid (97, 128), with postcranial adaptations to alternative types of orthogrady, such as forelimb-dominated behaviors (129) and terrestrial bipedalism (130). Even if not directly related to hominins (or modern hominoids), the locomotor adaptations of Oreopithecus, and other Miocene apes, are worthy of further research to understand the selection pressures that led to the (independent) emergence of modern hominoid positional behaviors.

To distinguish true locomotor adaptations from exaptations, current research efforts focus on plastic ecophenotypic traits, potentially denoting how fossil hominoids were actually moving. Bone is a living tissue, and growth is expected to occur in predictable ways that reflect loading patterns throughout life (131). Thus, cross-sectional and trabecular bone properties and their links to behavior are widely investigated (132, 133). Yet, experimental studies indicate that internal bone morphology does not necessarily match stereotypical loading patterns (134). Ample evidence suggests that irregular loading, even in low magnitude, can be more osteogenically potent than stereotypical loading (135). This may bias interpretations of individual fossils with a species-atypical loading pattern during life (e.g., because of an injury). Bone (re)modeling also does not consistently occur in response to changes in loading pattern: It can occur in ways that detract from, rather than enhance, function (136) and may manifest differentially across the skeleton (137). Incongruence also exists between actual bone performance and expectations based on aspects of internal morphology (138). Finally, there is a strong genetic component to the responsiveness of bone (re)modeling to loading (136), which is largely unknown for most species. The confidence with which internal bone structures can be used to retrodict behavior in fossil species remains a work in progress.

Competing hypotheses about the locomotor behavior immediately preceding hominin bipedalism include terrestrial knuckle walking (15), palmigrade quadrupedalism (93), and different types of arboreal (orthograde) behaviors such as climbing and suspension (7), vertical climbing (139), or arboreal bipedalism and suspension (104, 140). Miocene great apes can enlighten this question by helping to identify the polarity of evolutionary change preceding the Pan-Homo divergence (81, 82). For instance, if Pierolapithecus is interpreted as an orthograde ape without specific suspensory adaptations but retaining quadrupedal adaptations [see alternatives in (10)], then the orthograde body plan and ulnocarpal contact loss could be interpreted as an adaptation to vertical climbing, subsequently co-opted for suspension (19). Similarly, habitual bipedalism might have directly evolved from other orthograde behaviors without an intermediate stage of advanced suspension or specialized knuckle walking. Hence, Pierolapithecus complements previous hypotheses that biomechanical aspects of the lower limb during quadrupedalism and vertical climbing could be functionally preadaptive for bipedalism (39, 139).

A holistic view indicates that the Pan-Homo LCA was a Miocene ape with extant great apelike cognitive abilities, likely possessing a complex social structure and tool traditions (36, 38, 141). This ape would exhibit some degree of body size and canine sexual dimorphism (with large honing male canines) (15), indicating a polygynous sociosexual system (40). Based on Miocene apes and earliest hominins, it is also likely that the Pan-Homo LCA was orthograde and proficient at vertical climbing [see alternative interpretation based on Ardipithecus (33, 93)], but not necessarily adapted specifically for below-branch suspension or knuckle walking (9, 33). Chimpanzees seem to retain the Pan-Homo LCA plesiomorphic condition in some regards [e.g., brain and body size (38), vertebral counts (125), foot morphology (142)]. However, in others [e.g., interlimb (93), hand (9), pelvis (143) length proportions; femur morphology (89)], early hominins are more similar to generalized Miocene apes. These results further reinforce the idea that functional aspects of other locomotor types were co-opted for bipedalism during hominin origins.

The East Side Story scenario links the divergence of chimpanzees and humans to the rifting of East Africa, which would have triggered a vicariant speciation event from the ancestral Pan-Homo LCA (17). Chimpanzees would have remained frozen in time in their ancestral tropical forest environment, whereas humans would be the descendants of the group left behind on the east side of the Rift. Major climate and landscape changes would have then forced the earliest hominins to adapt to more open (grassland savanna) environments by acquiring bipedalismand the rest is history. Several decades after the proposal of this scenario, where do we stand?

The landscape of East Africa has dramatically changed during the past 10 million years because of tectonic events leading to specific climatic conditions and associated changes in vegetation structure, from mixed tropical forest to more heterogeneous and arid environments than elsewhere in tropical Africa (144, 145). The trend of progressive aridification did not culminate in the predominance of savanna environments until ~2.0 Maroughly coinciding with hominin brain size increase and the appearance of H. erectusand was punctuated by alternating episodes of extreme humidity and aridity, resulting in a fluctuating extension of forests through time (144, 145). Despite ongoing discussions about early hominin paleoenvironments (woodland with forest patches versus wooded savanna) (146), evidence from Miocene apes (30, 31) supports that the Pan-Homo LCA inhabited some kind of woodland. Therefore, it has been suggested that the Pan-Homo LCA was probably more omnivorous than chimpanzees (ripe fruit specialists) and likely fed both in trees and on the ground (33), in agreement with isotopic analyses for Ardipithecus ramidus (41).

Bipedalism would have emerged because of the selection pressures created by the progressive fragmentation of forested habitats and the need for terrestrial travel from one feeding patch to the next. Data on extant ape positional behaviors (Fig. 4) suggest that hominin terrestrial bipedalism originated as a posture rather than a means of travel on the ground (147) or in trees (140). Rose (39) proposed a long process of increasing commitment to bipedality in the transition to more complex open habitats throughout the Plio-Pleistocene, and Potts (148) argued that key stages in hominin evolution may relate to adaptive responses to cope with highly variable environments. The fossil and archaeological records provide a new twist to the order of evolutionary events in early hominin evolution. The remains of Orrorin and Ar. ramidus indicate that habitual terrestrial bipedalism, enhanced precision grasping, and loss of canine honing evolved at the dawn of the human lineage well before brain enlargement (9, 33, 89, 93). It was not until later in time [maybe starting with Australopithecus (149) and continuing with Homo], that some preexisting hand attributes were co-opted for purposive and systematic stone toolmaking in more encephalized hominins with more advanced cognitive abilities (38, 150).

Although one particular behavior can dominate the locomotor repertoire of a given species, the full positional repertoire (postural and locomotor behaviors) of living primates is diverse, complex, and not fully understood. For example, some locomotor behaviors are not totally comparable (e.g., monkey quadrupedalism versus African ape knuckle walking). Furthermore, comprehensive data are not yet available for some extant hominoids (e.g., Gorilla). Bipedalism did not appear de novo in hominins; it existed as a posture or locomotion within a broader Miocene ape positional repertoire. The combined evidence of Miocene apes and early hominins indicate that the locomotor repertoire of the Pan-Homo LCA likely included a combination of positional behaviors not represented among living primates. Over time, bipedal behaviors became the predominant activity within the repertoire of early hominins (and knuckle walking in the chimpanzee lineage). Locomotor behaviors (plus bipedal standing) in each taxon represent percentages of total positional behavior repertoire. (The full repertoire is not shown; hence, these do not add to 100%.) Data were taken from (92). Quadrupedalism includes Hunts categories quadrupedal walk and quadrupedal run, suspension includes suspensory, brachiate, clamber, and transfer. The locomotor repertoire compositions of the LCA and modern humans (Homo) are conjectural, for illustrative purposes.

That hominins continuously evolved since the Pan-Homo LCA is universally accepted, but the possibility that all living hominoids (including chimpanzees) experienced their own evolutionary histories is sometimes disregarded. Potts (151) suggested that the greater cognitive abilities of great apes originated to continue exploiting fruit supplies from densely forested environments in front of strong environmental variability. Coupled with locomotor adaptations (e.g., vertical climbing, suspension) enabling an efficient navigation through the canopy, this cognitive trap would consist of an adaptive feedback loop between diet, locomotion, cognition, and life history. Although hominids originated approximately during the Mid-Miocene Climatic Optimum (~17 to 15 Ma), their subsequent radiation from ~14 Ma onward paralleled a trend of climatic deterioration during the rest of the Miocene (152). Great apes might have initially thrived by evolving particular adaptations to more efficiently exploit their habitats, thereby occupying new adaptive peaks without abandoning the same area of the adaptive landscape broadly occupied by earlier stem hominoids. Nevertheless, this evolutionary strategy would become unsustainable once a particular paleoenvironmental threshold was surpassed. This could explain the fate of European dryopiths, which survived for some time under suboptimal conditions (despite the progressive trend of cooling and increased seasonality) until they vanished (94).

The dietary, locomotor, and cognitive specializations of late Miocene great apes would have hindered their shift into new adaptive peaks suitable for the more open environments toward the latest Miocene (153). The Miocene planet of the apes gave way to the time of the more generalist Old World monkeys, enabling their survival in a wider variety of seasonal habitats (30, 92, 154). The same specialization trap can explain the delayed retreat of pongines (and hylobatids) to southeastern Asia throughout the Plio-Pleistocene. The highly specialized orangutans remain extant, but not for long because their habitat continues to shrink. African apes could have partially overcome the specialization trap by evolving (perhaps in parallel) semiterrestrial adaptationsknuckle walking. Gorillas also expanded their dietary range (more folivorous) and enlarged their body size. Contrary to the view that gorillas are enlarged chimpanzees, morphometric analyses indicate that gorillas underwent their own evolutionary history, resulting in different ontogenetic trajectories (155, 156) and postcranial differences that cannot be explained by size-scaling effects (9, 143). Why, when, and how many times knuckle walking evolved is more difficult to explain than the origin of hominin bipedalism. Habitat fragmentation coupled with a higher reliance on arboreal feeding might be invoked (i.e., knuckle walking serves both terrestrial and arboreal locomotion). This idea is difficult to reconcile with the premise that continuous-canopy forests covered the tropical belt of central and western Africa since the Miocene, unless gorillas and chimpanzees evolved in less densely forested habitats (30, 31, 114) and retreated to tropical forests when outcompeted by hominins and/or cercopithecoids. Ironically, the same specializations that allowed great apes to survive despite major environmental challenges since the late Miocene might ultimately doom them to extinction.

Hominins might have escaped the great-ape specialization trap by evolving novel and more radical adaptations: bipedalism (another specialized orthograde locomotion), concomitant freeing of the hands, and subsequent enhanced manual dexterity, brain configuration, sociosexual behavior, and culturally mediated technology. Human evolution also reflects the progressive adaptation (biological first, cultural later) to ever-changing environments (39, 148). Some essential changes (upright posture, enhanced cognition) are just the continuation of a trend started in Miocene hominoids (19, 36, 151). While escaping from the great ape specialization trap, humans might have fallen into another evolutionary cul-de-sac, with current human activities and overpopulation leading the biosphere to a point beyond return (157). Will humans escape their own specialization trap?

Fossils uniquely inform deep-time evolutionary studies, which is essential to plan for the future (158). However, we must be aware of the many existing limitations and the gaps in our knowledge. For example, we need more fossils because we are likely missing vastly more than what we have. More fieldwork is necessary to find fossil apes close to the gorilla or chimpanzee lineages, and it is essential to extend such efforts to unexplored or undersampled areas (Fig. 1). It is also essential to continue developing tools of phylogenetic inference. Bayesian approaches are promising, but uncertainty remains about their applicability to morphological data (159). Improvements in the treatment of continuous characters and recent methodological advances for analyzing three-dimensional geometric morphometric data within a cladistic framework (in combination with traditional characters) are promising for reconstructing fossil hominoid phylogeny (160). The oldest (recently retrieved) ancient DNA is ~1 Ma (161). Paleoproteomics could be a complementary solution because it has enabled sampling further back in time up to ~2 Ma, recently confirming the pongine status of Gigantopithecus (162). Future technological advances in paleoproteomics could potentially help to answer key questions by retrieving paleoproteomes from Miocene apes.

Locomotor reconstructions of the Pan-Homo LCA and other fossil hominoids are seriously hampered by the lack of current analogs. Washburn spotted the fundamental limitation: It is not possible to bring the past into the laboratory. No one can see a walking Australopithecus [(163), p. 67]. Such inferences rely on morphofunctional assumptions of bone, joint, or muscle function, but experimentally derived biomechanical data are required to test these assumptions and provide reliable inferences from fossils. Technological advances now facilitate noninvasive kinematic data collection from animals in their natural environments (164). In turn, experimental and morphological information should be integrated to better predict the locomotion of fossil hominoids. Forward dynamic simulations offer a powerful pathway for predicting de novo movements in fossil species while iterating possible effects of morphology and soft tissue (165).

Humans are storytellers: Theories of human evolution often resemble anthropogenic narratives that borrow the structure of a heros journey to explain essential aspects such as the origins of erect posture, the freeing of the hands, or brain enlargement (166). Intriguingly, such narratives have not drastically changed since Darwin (166). We must be aware of confirmation biases and ad hoc interpretations by researchers aiming to confer their new fossil the starring role within a preexisting narrative. Evolutionary scenarios are appealing because they provide plausible explanations based on current knowledge, but unless grounded in testable hypotheses, they are no more than just-so stories (167).

Many uncertainties persist about fossil apes, and the day in which the paleobiology of extinct species can be undisputedly reconstructed is still far away. However, current disagreements regarding ape and human evolution would be much more informed if, together with early hominins and living apes, Miocene apes were also included in the equation. This approach will allow us to better discern primitive and derived traits, the common from the specific, or the unique. This is the role of fossil apes in human evolution.

C. Darwin, The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex (Vol. I) (John Murray, 1871).

T. H. Huxley, Evidence As To Mans Place in Nature (Williams and Norgate, 1863).

M. Nakatsukasa, S. Almcija, D. R. Begun, in The Evolution of the Primate Hand: Anatomical, Developmental, Functional, and Paleontological Evidence, L. T. Kivell, P. Lemelin, G. B. Richmond, D. Schmitt, Eds. (Springer, 2016), pp. 485514.

M. Cartmill, in Functional Vertebrate Morphology, M. Hildebrand, D. Bramble, K. Liem, D. Wake, Eds. (Belknap Press, 1985), pp. 7388.

D. R. Pilbeam, D. E. Lieberman, in Chimpanzees and Human Evolution, M. N. Muller, R. W. Wrangham, D. R. Pilbeam, Eds. (The Belknap Press of Harvard Univ. Press, 2017), pp. 22141.

R. Wrangham, D. Pilbeam, in All Apes Great and Small, B. M. F. Galdikas, N. E. Briggs, L. K. Sheeran, G. L. Shapiro, J. Goodall, Eds. (Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers, 2001), pp. 517.

P. Andrews, An Apes View of Human Evolution (Cambridge Univ. Press, 2016).

C. Linnaeus, Systema Naturae per Regna Tria Naturae, Secundum Classes, Ordines, Genera, Species, cum Characteribus, Differentiis. Synonymis, Locis (Laurentius Salvius, Holmiae, 1758), vol. Tomus I. Editio Decima, reformata.

R. D. Martin, Primate Origins and Evolution: A Phylogenetic Reconstruction (Chapman and Hall London, 1990).

S. Almcija, C. C. Sherwood, in The Nervous Systems of Non-Human Primates, vol. 3 of Evolution of Nervous Systems, J. Kaas, Ed. (Elsevier, ed. 2, 2017), pp. 299315.

M. Rose, The process of bipedalization in hominids in Origine(s) de la bipdie chez les hominids, Y. Coppens, B. Senut, Eds. (CNRS, Paris, 1991), pp. 3748.

C. Darwin, On the Origin of Species. Or the Preservation of Favoured Races in the Struggle for Life (John Murray, 1859).

E. Haeckel, Natrliche Schpfungsgeschichte (Georg Reimer, 1868).

G. Elliot Smith, The Evolution of Man: Essays (Oxford Univ. Press, 1924).

H. Weinert, Ursprung der Menschheit: ber den engeren Anschluss des Menschengeschlechts an die Menschenaffen (Ferdinand Enke, 1932).

W. Hennig, Phylogenetic Systematics (Univ. Illinois Press, 1966).

R. Tuttle, in Phylogeny of the Primates, W. P. Luckett, F. S. Szalay, Eds. (Springer, 1975), pp. 447480.

P. Andrews, T. Harrison, in Interpreting the Past: Essays on Human, Primate, and Mammal Evolution, D. E. Lieberman, R. J. Smith, J. Kelley, Eds. (Brill Academic, 2005).

D. R. Begun, The Real Planet of the Apes: A New Story of Human Origins (Princeton Univ. Press, 2016).

K. D. Pugh, The phylogenetic relationships of Middle-Late Miocene apes: Implications for early human evolution, thesis, The Graduate Center, City University of New York (2020).

D. R. Begun, in Phylogeny of the Neogene Hominoid Primates of Eurasia, vol. 2 of Hominoid Evolution and Climatic Change in Europe, L. de Bonis, G. D. Koufos, P. Andrews, Eds. (Cambridge Univ. Press, 2001), pp. 231253.

Read the original post:
Fossil apes and human evolution - Science Magazine

Do You Prefer Cats or Dogs? Why Self-expression Increases Giving – Knowledge@Wharton – Knowledge@Wharton

Do you prefer dogs or cats? Vanilla or chocolate? Winter or summer? The answers to these simple questions reveal a little something about who we are and what we like. We want to answer them because theyre fun, and because it broadcasts a bit of information about our personality, our values, and our desires.

But there is also a serious side to these questions, which Wharton marketing professor Jonah Berger explains in a new paper titled, Penny for Your Preferences: Leveraging Self-Expression to Encourage Small Pro-Social Gifts. The paper looks at how businesses can use this intrinsic desire for self-expression to get consumers to give more money, whether its tipping the barista a little extra or donating more dollars to a charitable cause. He and his co-authors call it the dueling preferences approach, which frames the act of giving as a choice.

The co-authors of the paper, which was published in the Journal of Marketing, are Jacqueline Rifkin, assistant marketing professor at the University of Missouri-Kansas Citys Henry W. Bloch School of Management, and Katherine M. Du, assistant marketing professor at the University of Wisconsin-Milwaukees Lubar School of Business.

Berger joined Knowledge@Wharton to talk about the study. Listen to the podcast at the top of this page, or read an edited transcript of the conversation below.

Knowledge@Wharton: How does answering questions about personal preferences make people want to give more money?

Jonah Berger: Theres a fundamental question that many organizations or people have thought about, which is, how do we increase pro-social behavior? If Im the Red Cross, how do I get more donations? If Im a barista at a coffee shop or a waiter at a restaurant, how do I get people to tip me? If Im trying to raise money for a museum, how do I go about doing that? Its obviously very hard. Lots of people mean to donate, they want to donate, but there are lots of causes and lots of things going on. In the end, it often doesnt happen.

We all love self-expression. We do it all the time through our cars and clothes and music. [My co-authors and I] wondered whether we could leverage this tendency and this desire for self-expression to encourage pro-social behavior. Part of this idea actually started from something we saw in coffee shops. You might walk into a coffee shop, and rather than having a tip jar there for you to drop in a buck or two, instead there are two jars with pictures on them. One might say dog and one says cat. They might say vanilla ice cream or chocolate ice cream. Or they might say Star Wars or Star Trek.

We all love self-expression. We do it all the time through our cars and clothes and music.

Coffee shops are using that approach for a reason. They think its engaging their customers in some way and hopefully increasing tips. But we wondered, does it work? There are many things that businesses try that dont work, so does this actually work? Does it work all the time? Can we apply this more broadly? Knowing that people drop in a couple of extra cents for cats versus dogs is nice for a coffee shop. But if Im the leader of a big nonprofit, or Im trying to raise money for a foundation, could something like this be useful for me? Is it just something for coffee shops, or can it tell us something broader about human behavior and ways that organizations can leverage these insights to increase pro-social behavior?

Knowledge@Wharton: How did you study this? It seems subjective, so how did you make this analytical and objective?

Berger: Ill give you a couple of examples. One experiment we did was very much in the exact setting we talked about: a local coffee shop. We went in and, for different periods of time, we had different tipping situations. Sometimes there would be a single jar that would say tips. Other times, we randomly manipulated whether it had just a jar or this idea of dueling preferences these two things that customers could vote on based on their opinions. They could choose cats or dogs through their tipping. We manipulated the time of day across multiple days, counterbalancing for everything almost like an A/B test to get a sense of what affects donations.

You could say, Well, hold on. Youre asking people to make a choice. Cats and dogs have nothing to do with tipping. Maybe its going to decrease donations. Maybe people are going to feel overwhelmed. Theyre not going to make a choice. But thats not what happened. Giving people a choice mattered. Just making two jars and putting cat and dog on them led people to tip more than twice as much compared to a tip jar.

It wasnt just restricted to tipping. We did a very similar experiment with donations to the American Red Cross. Rather than simply asking for donations and thats what we did for some people some people were asked to donate by voting. Would you prefer chocolate ice cream or vanilla ice cream? Again, chocolate and vanilla ice cream have nothing to do with the American Red Cross. You could say, Well, hold on. Wont people think this is frivolous or doesnt matter? Its not going to help. But thats not what occurred. In fact, just the opposite: It increased donations by 28%.

In a variety of different contexts, we can use dueling preferences. Its giving people a self-expressive choice as a way to motivate action and, in this case, motivate giving.

If I were a marketer, if I were a manager, if I were a charity director, I would think about how to harness self-expression.

Knowledge@Wharton: Were you surprised by the experiment results, or did they line up with what we already know in the literature about identity and consumer behavior?

Berger: I was surprised by the size of these results. This isnt a couple of pennies here and there. A 28% increase to the American Red Cross is a big deal. Thats a lot of money for that organization. I was certainly surprised by the size of the effect, but it was also interesting to see when this happens and why it happens. Im not suggesting just to give anybody a choice or that any choice will work. It really has to be a way for people to express their preference. It has to be something that they care about, and they feel that expressing their preference on that dimension is diagnostic of who they are. Sports rivalries are the same way, right? Maybe its the Yankees and the Red Sox. Thats something people feel very strongly about. They want to share their opinion.

Its about understanding the context. Pick a choice that the audience cares about and feels self-expressive. Caring about chocolate and vanilla ice cream is not the most important domain in the world, but its something people feel says something about them. The same with dogs and cats. There are cat people and there are dog people who feel like it says something about them. We can use that to motivate behavior even in an unrelated domain.

Theres lots of research that clearly shows that if youre the type of person who drives a BMW, thats a desirable identity for you. Youre going to pay more for a BMW than someone who doesnt hold that [identity]. Its clear we care about identity. Its clear that identity motivates behavior.

Knowledge@Wharton: Given this information, what can marketers, managers, or even charity directors do to help increase giving?

Berger: I think the place to start is to stop just thinking about you. Your cause is very important, but think about your audience. What this research shows is, yes, if Im the American Red Cross or whatever organization it might be I can go out there and say, This is an important problem. Please donate money to this problem. And I will get people to donate. I will get a set of people who have donated in the past to donate to my cause.

Those arent small circles of people. But if I want to move beyond those circles of people, I have to think [bigger]. There are other important causes. Theres the environment, and theres cancer theres a variety of things people could give their money to. I have to think beyond just my cause and people who inherently believe in that cause to begin with, and start thinking about, Well, what do those folks care about?

Even people that may care about the cause to begin with whats a way to motivate them to give? This isnt just about making it a game, though it does feel a little bit like a game. Its really about allowing them to express themselves. Maybe your local grocery store does this competition where they say, Collect receipts, give them to your local school, and the school that gets the most receipts gets a big donation from the grocery store. Thats not just allowing people to express themselves, but to compete. It allows those expressions to be public signals of the self.

How can I motivate my audience to give by providing them an opportunity to express their preferences?

If I were a marketer, if I were a manager, if I were a charity director, I would think about how to harness self-expression. I would think about the right opportunity to give people the right choice. How can I motivate my audience to give by providing them an opportunity to express their preferences?

Knowledge@Wharton: Whats next for this line of research?

Berger: One thing Ive thought a bit about lately is the value of asking questions rather than making statements. I talk about this a bit in my most recent book, The Catalyst. We did some research on it here, and were doing some research on it more generally. But questions are really powerful in a number of ways.

Often, when we want to persuade people, we think that telling them to do what we want making a statement, if you will is the best way to get them to take action. But people often push back on statements. Questions do a number of interesting things. First, they allow us to collect information. If you ask questions, you can better understand the people youre communicating with, the people youre trying to persuade. Second, it gives people some freedom and autonomy. Just like we talked about today, it allows them to express themselves. It allows them to participate.

If youre a boss trying to get people to stay late after work, and you tell them what you need, they may push back. Instead, if you say, What kind of company do we want to be, a good one or a great one? And they answer, A great one. And then you ask, What can we do to get there? They think about it, and they give you some answers, and you adopt those answers. Now, theyre more brought into the process. I think questions can be a great way to make people feel like they have a role in the process, which makes them much more likely to be engaged and to help [achieve] a desired outcome.

Read the original post:
Do You Prefer Cats or Dogs? Why Self-expression Increases Giving - Knowledge@Wharton - Knowledge@Wharton

From laughing and music to yelling and crying 6-year-old slain during West Side car meet – San Antonio Express-News

A 6-year-old girl was killed Sunday night when gunfire erupted during a noisy gathering of car enthusiasts at a strip mall parking lot on the West Side.

A 23-year-old man opened fire around 11 p.m., the result of an argument whose cause investigators were still trying to determine a day later, police said.

The girl, Saryah Perez, was at the gathering with her mother and was in the rear seat of a car when a round struck her in the upper torso, Police Chief William McManus said Monday.

She was transported to University Hospital, where she died.

Andrew Ray Elizondo, 23, was arrested Monday afternoon and charged with capital murder in the girls death.

Elizondo was taken into custody without incident around 2:30 p.m., McManus said. The suspect was an acquaintance of Saryahs mother, but that appeared to play no role in the shooting, the chief said.

This was not a domestic violence-related incident, McManus said.

Andrew Ray Elizondo

Its a random act by a reckless, heartless individual who could not have cared less where those bullets wound up, he said. This is all about human behavior, and someone who would do something like this I dont even have words for.

The girls mother, Kassandra Mendoza, was grazed in the back by shrapnel and drove to a nearby convenience store to ask for help, police said.

McManus said thats when the mother realized her daughter had been shot.

Members of a car club had gathered in the parking lot of a retail strip on the south side of Commerce, west of 24th, near Our Lady of the Lake University. The cluster of businesses there includes a Family Dollar store, a mobile phone shop, a furniture and appliance rental center, two loan stores and a Bank of America cash machine.

Roy Alderete, 35, said he witnessed the shooting and its aftermath. He spoke with a San Antonio Express-News reporter Monday afternoon at a convenience store on West 24th, diagonally across from the retail strip.

Kassandra Mendoza, the mother of Saryah Perez, is comforted by SAISD Chief William McManus during a rally Monday evening to honor 6 year-old Saryah who was shot and killed Sunday night.

Late Sunday night, Alderete said, he saw cars crowded into the parking lot. Then he heard a couple of shots, he said.

Next thing you know, theres just these cars flying everywhere, Alderete said.

One of the cars, a red sedan, pulled up at the convenience store, he said. Two women, a man and a girl were inside. The girl was in a car seat in the back, he said. Music blared from the vehicle.

I thought they were going to stop and come in the store, Alderete said.

Then, he said, one of the women checked on the girl and realized she had been shot.

It just went from laughing and music to yelling and crying, Alderete said.

One of the women pulled the girl from the car, laid her on the ground and tried to revive her, without success. She had lost a lot of blood, he said.

Alderete snapped pictures of the scene with his mobile phone. One of them showed a kneeling woman cradling a girl in her arms.

On Monday evening, Mendoza returned with family members to the spot where she had held her daughters lifeless body less than 24 hours earlier.

More than 200 people attended the vigil amid a strong police presence.

Kassandra Mendoza and Julio Garcia, parents of Saryah Perez during a rally Monday evening to honor their 6 year-old who was shot and killed Sunday night.

Kassandra Mendoza and Julio Garcia, parents of Saryah Perez and other family members cry during a rally Monday evening to honor their 6 year-old who was shot and killed Sunday night.

Supporters hold hands during a rally Monday evening to honor 6 year-old Saryah Perez who was shot and killed Sunday night.

Joe Albert Montes leads a prayer during a rally Monday evening to honor 6 year-old Saryah Perez who was shot and killed Sunday night.

Kassandra Mendoza and Julio Garcia, parents of Saryah Perez and other family members cry during a rally Monday evening to honor their 6 year-old who was shot and killed Sunday night.

People pray during a rally Monday evening to honor 6 year-old Saryah Perez who was shot and killed Sunday night.

Regina Navarro speaks during a rally Monday evening to honor 6 year-old Saryah Perez who was shot and killed Sunday night.

Well wishers surround and pray for Kassandra Mendoza during a rally Monday evening to honor her 6 year-old Saryah Perez who was shot and killed Sunday night.

David Segura prays during a rally Monday evening to honor 6 year-old Saryah Perez who was shot and killed Sunday night.

Kassandra Mendoza and Julio Garcia, parents of Saryah Perez and other family members cry during a rally Monday evening to honor their 6 year-old who was shot and killed Sunday night.

Supporters fill the lot at 24th and Commerce during a rally Monday evening to honor 6 year-old Saryah Perez who was shot and killed Sunday night.

Supporters fill the lot at 24th and Commerce during a rally Monday evening to honor 6 year-old Saryah Perez who was shot and killed Sunday night.

The parents of 6 year old Saryah Perez, Kassandra Mendoza and Julio Garcia (center) are surrounded by supporters during a rally Monday evening to honor 6 year-old Saryah who was shot and killed Sunday night.

Richard Gallardo holds a sign during a rally Monday evening to honor 6 year-old Saryah Perez who was shot and killed Sunday night.

Well wishers surround and pray for Kassandra Mendoza during a rally Monday evening to honor her 6 year-old Saryah Perez who was shot and killed Sunday night.

A woman collapses after the shooting, which took place at a strip mall parking lot on Commerce, west of 24th.

Mendoza wept uncontrollably alongside her mother and grandmother as Regina Navarro, a minister with River of Life Church, raised her arms and prayed over the family.

People joined in prayer, released silver and purple balloons, and held neon pink and yellow signs that read Stop the Violence, and Put down the guns, dont be a coward.

Navarro said Mendoza told her the fight started with a disagreement between a group of men and Mendozas boyfriend. She said the men, one of whom had previously dated Mendoza, confronted Mendoza and pulled her current boyfriend out of a vehicle.

Navarro said the men started beating Mendozas boyfriend until he got in the car with Mendoza and her daughter and drove off. When the car reached the street, one of the men pulled out a gun and fired at it, Navarro said.

We need to make a difference in San Antonio, Navarro said. Its just destroying everything that we worked for.

At car club meetups, people hang out, show off vintage automobiles and trade tips on maintenance and modifications. These are social events. McManus said thats how Sundays gathering started out. He called it a meeting of a legitimate car club.

Police have had problems in the last year with a different kind of meetup, where people race their cars, block streets or intersections, damage property and sometimes clash with police officers.

Kassandra Mendoza, the mother of Saryah Perez, is comforted by SAISD Chief William McManus during a rally Monday evening to honor 6 year-old Saryah who was shot and killed Sunday night.

Between Sept. 16 and May 5, San Antonio police recorded 24 encounters with such groups.

A task force assembled by McManus to address the problem made 116 felony and misdemeanor arrests in that time period.

Mariah Medina, a San Antonio Police Department spokeswoman, said the group that met Sunday night had not been on the departments radar.

The scene of the shooting is in City Council District 5, represented by Shirley Gonzales.

Gonzales said she lives a few blocks from the retail strip and heard gunshots around 11 p.m. Sunday, followed by sirens.

This isnt the first time since I took office that a child has been killed by a stray bullet, Gonzales said. Off the top of my head, I can think of at least five instances of children being murdered.

Gun violence in this neighborhood is just out of control, she said. Incidents like these dont get as much attention as mass shootings because they are individual incidents, but they are equally as devastating.

The rest is here:
From laughing and music to yelling and crying 6-year-old slain during West Side car meet - San Antonio Express-News

What first set humans apart from other animals? – Quartz Africa

How did human uniqueness first evolve among our ancestors, setting us apart from other animals? That is a question many archaeologists are grappling with by investigating early records of art, language, food preparation, ornaments, and symbols. How our ancestors treated and mourned the dead can also offer crucial clues, helping to reveal when we first developed the abstract thinking needed to fully grasp the concept of death.

Now we have discovered a 78,000-year-old human burial at a cave in the tropical coast of eastern Africa, which provides tantalizing evidence about our ancestors treatment of the dead. Our new study, published in Nature, describes the burial of a two-and-a-half to three-year-old child, nicknamed Mtoto (Swahili for child), at the Panga ya Saidi archaeological site in Kenya. It is the earliest known Homo sapiens burial in Africa.

The excavations began in 2010. So far, they have revealed a record of human occupation from 78,000 to 500 years ago, covering the Middle Stone Age and Later Stone Age periods of African archaeology. Mtotos burial lay towards the base of the excavation site and was first recognized because it contained sediment of a different color from the surroundings.

The initial examination revealed highly degraded bone. We quickly realized that the material was so fragile that standard excavation techniques were not suitable. Instead the whole burial pit was removed as a single block of sediment and sent to the National Research Centre on Human Evolution (CENIEH) in Burgos, Spain.

Several months of painstaking laboratory excavation revealed Mtoto, lying on their right side and with the knees drawn towards the chest. The skeleton was reasonably intact, which alongside detailed analysis of the surrounding sediment implied that the body decomposed within a filled grave. The displacements of some bones suggested that Mtotos upper body was either tightly shrouded in some sort of perishable material, presumably hide or vegetation, or that the grave was densely packed with sediment during the burial.

Intriguingly, there is also evidence that Mtotos head may have been supported by perishable material in the grave. It was found rotated relative to the body, a common occurrence when pillowing decomposesleaving a void. Clearly, Mtoto was carefully placed within a grave, probably with the upper body shrouded and the head pillowed, prior to burial. The evidence suggests that Mtotos body was disposed of deliberately, with some form of community involvement or funerary rite. Certainly the body wasnt abandoned or accidentally buried by geological processes such as a flood.

What can this tell us about our ancestors? In Eurasia, both Homo Sapiens and Neanderthals commonly buried their dead in residential sites from at least 120,000 years ago. Why does the oldest burial in Africa occur so much later, given the continents centrality to the emergence of modern human behavior? One possibility is that prior to 78,000 years ago, African populations treated their dead differently.

There is some evidence that earlier populations in Africa may have removed the flesh from key body parts, notably the cranium, and stored only the bones. This process has been referred to as defleshing and curation. Cutmarks and polishing on three 150,000-year-old skulls found at Herto, Ethiopia, supports this possibility. It is possible that this special treatment of the dead was associated with grief or mourning.

We may also be looking for early human bodies in the wrong places. Most archaeological excavations occur at residential sites. If earlier cultures disposed of bodies away from these areas, they would be archaeologically invisible. For example, bodies may have been left in natural places such as cave fissures or hollows, a practice known as funerary caching.

The precise cultural significance of funerary caching is unclear, but the practice appears to be ancient. A large concentration of hominin bones dated to 430,000 years ago was also found at Sima de los Huesos (Pit of the Bones) in Atapuerca, Spain.

Prior to the discovery of Mtoto, the earliest known African burials were at Taramsa, Egypt (69,000 years ago) and Border Cave, South Africa (74,000 years ago). The Taramsa child was found in a pit, initially dug to mine rock for stone tool production. Consequently this site may be viewed as a late example of funerary caching. The Border Cave infant was excavated in 1941 and, unlike with Mtoto, no information about the position of the remains are available. This makes it impossible to unequivocally describe the evidence from Border Cave as a burial.

But taken together, the evidence possibly suggests that African funerary practices changed over time. It may indicate a shift, sometime between ~150,000 and ~80,000 years ago, from the defleshing and curation seen at Herto, to funerary caching and burials at Panga ya Saidi, Taramsa, and Border Cave. It is also striking that all of these sites contain younger individuals. Possibly the bodies of children received special treatment in this ancient period.

This article is republished from The Conversation under a Creative Commons license. Read the original article.

Sign up to the Quartz Africa Weekly Brief here for news and analysis on African business, tech, and innovation in your inbox.

Read more from the original source:
What first set humans apart from other animals? - Quartz Africa

Science Papers Examine Factors Shaping SARS-CoV-2 Spread, Give Insight Into Bacterial Evolution – GenomeWeb

By sequencing nearly 4,000 SARS-CoV-2 genomes collected in Washington State last year, a group led by Fred Hutchinson Cancer Research Center investigators has found that human behavior, rather than different viral lineages, was mostly responsible for shaping the course of the pandemic in the region. As reported in Science Translational Medicine, the researchers find that cases of infection with the 614D variant initially dominated in Washington State, but were later taken over the potentially more transmissible 614G variant. However, the trends for 614G and 614D cases appeared to be explained by differences in when action to curb the spread ofSARS-CoV-2 were taken on a county level. Additionally, while higher viral loads were observed in patients infected with the 614G variant, the scientists did not find evidence that the variant impacts clinical severity or patient outcomes.

Using a novel hierarchical phylogenomic approach, a team led by scientists from the University of Bristol has identified the root of the bacteria tree and gained new insights into early bacterial evolution. In their study, which appears in this week's Science, the investigators note that tracing billions of years of bacterial evolution back to the root has been difficult because standard phylogenetic models do not account for the full range of evolutionary processes that shape bacterial genomes. Standard rooting approaches also typically use an outgroup, which act a reference point for evolutionary analyses but have the potential to distort within-species relationships. Using a technique that explicitly uses information from gene duplications and losses within a genome, as well as gene transfers between genomes, they were able to root the bacterial tree without including an archaeal outgroup. Their analysis puts the root of the bacteria tree between the major clades Terrabacteria and Gracilicutes and suggests that the last bacterial common ancestor was a complex double-membraned cell capable of motility and chemotaxis that possessed a CRISPR-Cas system. The researchers also uncover a major role for vertical gene transmission in bacterial evolution.

More here:
Science Papers Examine Factors Shaping SARS-CoV-2 Spread, Give Insight Into Bacterial Evolution - GenomeWeb

Dogs’ aggressive behavior towards humans often caused by fear. Here’s why – Hindustan Times

A recent study encompassing some 9,000 dogs conducted at the University of Helsinki demonstrated that fearfulness, age, breed, the company of other members of the same species, and the owner's previous experience of dogs were all associated with aggressive behaviour towards humans.

The findings published in the journal Scientific Reports can potentially provide tools for understanding and preventing aggressive behaviour. Aggressive behaviour in dogs can include growling, barking, snapping and biting. These gestures are part of normal canine communication, and they also occur in non-aggressive situations, such as during play. However, aggressive behaviour can be excessive, making the dog a health threat to both humans and other animals.

"Understanding the factors underlying aggressive behaviour is important. In what kinds of circumstances does aggressive behaviour occur and what is the dog's motive for such behaviour? In normal family dogs, aggressive behaviour is often unwanted, while some dogs with official duties are expected to have the capacity for aggressiveness. At the same time, aggressiveness can be caused by welfare issues, such as chronic pain," says doctoral researcher Salla Mikkola from the University of Helsinki.

The canine gene research group active at the University of Helsinki surveyed connections between aggressive behaviour and several potential risk factors with the help of a dataset encompassing more than 9,000 dogs, a sample from a larger dataset from a behavioural survey dataset of nearly 14,000 dogs. The study investigated aggressiveness towards both dog owners and unfamiliar human beings. Dogs were classified as aggressive if they growled often and/or had attempted to snap at or bite a human at least occasionally in the situations described in the survey.

"Dogs' fearfulness had a strong link to aggressive behaviour, with fearful dogs many times more likely to behave aggressively. Moreover, older dogs were more likely to behave aggressively than younger ones. One of the potential reasons behind this can be pain caused by a disease. Impairment of the senses can contribute to making it more difficult to notice people approaching, and dogs' responses to sudden situations can be aggressive," Mikkola adds.

Small dogs are more likely to behave aggressively than mid-sized and large dogs, but their aggressive behaviour is not necessarily considered as threatening as that of large dogs. Consequently, their behaviour is not addressed. In addition, the study found that male dogs were more aggressive than females. However, sterilisation had no effect on aggressive behaviour.

The first dogs of dog owners were more likely to behave aggressively compared to dogs whose owners had previous experience of dogs. The study also indicated that dogs that spend time in the company of other dogs behave less aggressively than dogs that live without other dogs in the household. While this phenomenon has been observed in prior research, the causality remains unclear.

"In the case of dogs prone to aggressive behaviour in the first instance, owners may not necessarily wish to take a risk of conflicts with another dog," Mikkola muses. Significant differences in aggressive behaviour between breeds. Differences in the aggressiveness of various dog breeds can point to a genetic cause.

"In our dataset, the Long-Haired Collie, Poodle (Toy, Miniature and Medium) and Miniature Schnauzer were the most aggressive breeds. Previous studies have shown fearfulness in Long-Haired Collies, while the other two breeds have been found to express aggressive behaviour towards unfamiliar people. As expected, the popular breeds of Labrador Retriever and Golden Retriever were at the other extreme. People who are considering getting a dog should familiarise themselves with the background and needs of the breed. As for breeders, they should also pay attention to the character of dam candidates, since both fearfulness and aggressive behaviour are inherited," says Professor Hannes Lohi from the University of Helsinki.

Follow this link:
Dogs' aggressive behavior towards humans often caused by fear. Here's why - Hindustan Times